Experiential Gifts Foster Stronger Social
Relationships than Material Gifts
CINDY CHAN
CASSIE MOGILNER
Interpersonal relationships are essential to well-being, and gifts are often given to
cultivate these relationships. To inform gift givers of what to give and to gain in-
sight into the connecting function of gifts, this research investigates what type of
gift is better at strengthening relationships according to gift recipients—material
gifts (objects for recipients to keep) or experiential gifts (events for recipients to
live through). Experiments examining actual gift exchanges in real-life relation-
ships reveal that experiential gifts produce greater improvements in relationship
strength than material gifts, regardless of whether the gift giver and recipient con-
sume the gift together. The relationship improvements that recipients derive from
experiential gifts stem from the intensity of emotion that is evoked when they con-
sume the gifts, rather than when the gifts are received. Giving experiential gifts is
thus identified as a highly effective form of prosocial spending.
Keywords: gift giving, experiential purchases, material purchases, emotion,
relationships
E
ach year is replete with occasions to give gifts. From
birthdays to religious holidays, Valentine’s Day to
Father’s Day, these occasions are fraught with the ques-
tion: What to give?! Should you give your dad a designer
tie or golf lessons? Would giving your spouse a watch or
concert tickets spark greater affection? Would a set of
wine glasses or a wine tasting better cement your friend-
ship with your favorite colleague? And, ultimately, why
would one of these gifts strengthen the relationship more
than the other?
With the average American household spending almost
2% of its annual income on gifts (US Bureau of Labor
Statistics 2013), and with gift-giving occasions serving as
great opportunities (and liabilities) for relationship build-
ing, these are surprisingly consequential decisions. Indeed,
interpersonal relationships are essential to well-being
(Baumeister and Leary 1995; Clark and Lemay 2010;
Mogilner 2010; Reis, Collins, and Berscheid 2000), and
gifts serve as a means to foster these important connections
(Algoe, Haidt, and Gable 2008; Dunn et al. 2008b; Ruth,
Otnes, and Brunel 1999; Sherry 1983). To help inform gift
givers of what to give and to gain insight into the interper-
sonal benefits of gifts, this research adopts the gift recipi-
ents’ perspective and experimentally investigates which
type of gift is more effective at strengthening their relation-
ship with their gift giver—material gifts (objects for the re-
cipients to keep) or experiential gifts (events for the
recipients to live through). And why?
MATERIAL VERSUS EXPERIENTIAL GIFTS
Borrowing Van Boven and Gilovich’s (2003) definition
of material and experiential purchases, we define material
Cindy Chan ([email protected]) is an assistant professor of
marketing at the UTSC Department of Management and the Rotman
School of Management, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON M1C 1A4.
Cassie Mogilner (cassie.holmes@anderson.ucla.edu) is an associate
professor of marketing at the UCLA Anderson School of Management,
University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90024. This article is based on
a portion of the first author’s dissertation. The authors greatly appreciate
the support and facilities provided by the Wharton Behavioral Lab and the
financial support provided by Wharton’s Dean’s Research Fund, as well
as the research assistance provided by Christina Gizzo and Karishma
Antia. The authors are also grateful to have enjoyed the connecting power
of experiential gifts in their own relationships, particularly with their hus-
bands, Tom and Rob.
Ann McGill and Darren Dahl served as editors for this article. Darren
Dahl also served as associate editor.
Advance Access publication December 11, 2016
V
C
The Author 2016. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of Journal of Consumer Research, Inc.
All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected] Vol. 00 November 2016
DOI: 10.1093/jcr/ucw067
1
Journal of Consumer Research Advance Access published December 29, 2016
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
gifts as objects to be kept in the recipient’s possession
(e.g., jewelry or electronic gadgets) and experiential gifts
as an event that the recipient lives through (e.g., concert
tickets or a photography lesson).
The research comparing material and experiential pur-
chases to date has focused on the effects of making these
purchases for oneself, finding that buying an experience is
typically more personally beneficial than buying a material
good (Gilovich, Kumar, and Jampol 2015). Compared to
possessions, experiences lead to greater satisfaction (Carter
and Gilovich 2010), less regret (Rosenzweig and Gilovich
2012), and greater happiness (Van Boven and Gilovich
2003), especially when the outcome of the experience is
positive (Nicolao, Irwin, and Goodman 2009). The benefits
of acquiring an experience over a possession stem from the
fact that experiences are more likely to be shared with
others (Caprariello and Reis 2013), contribute more to
one’s sense of self (Carter and Gilovich 2012), are more
unique (Rosenzweig and Gilovich 2012), and are harder to
compare against alternatives (Carter and Gilovich 2010).
Although prior research offers guidance on whether to buy
experiences or material goods to improve one’s own well-
being, the question of what to buy to strengthen one’s rela-
tionships with others remains unanswered. Would giving
something to do or something to keep forge a stronger so-
cial bond?
It turns out that people are more inclined to give material
gifts. In a survey we conducted among 219 gift givers
(66% female; ages 18–74, M ¼ 34.68), 78% reported hav-
ing most recently given a gift that was material. This ten-
dency is consistent with the argument that giving a gift that
is durable will leave a lasting impression (Ariely 2011).
A pilot study we conducted around Father’s Day, how-
ever, hints that this tendency to give material gifts might
be misguided. Recipients of Father’s Day gifts (N ¼ 42;
ages 48–75; M ¼ 55.05) participated in a two-part survey:
one completed the week before Father’s Day and one the
week after. Both before and after Father’s Day, dads rated
the strength of their relationship with their child (1 ¼ feel
extremely distant and disconnected, 9 ¼ feel extremely
close and connected); any change would reflect the impact
of receiving the gift on the relationship. Dads also rated to
what extent the gift they received was material (1 ¼ not at
all, 7 ¼ completely) and experiential (1 ¼ not at all, 7 ¼
completely). A multiple regression analysis predicting
change in relationship strength showed that gifts that were
more experiential strengthened dads’ relationships with
their children (b ¼ 0.16, SE ¼ 0.07, t(39) ¼ 2.21, p ¼ .03,
d ¼ .71), whereas the material nature of the gift did not
(b ¼ –0.03, SE ¼ 0.07, t(39) ¼ –0.39, p ¼ .70, d ¼ .12). It
was not that experiential gifts were more likely to be given
in initially stronger relationships, since the material and ex-
periential gift ratings were unrelated to relationship
strength before Father’s Day (p’s
> .43). These results
were corroborated by a second pilot study conducted
following Mother’s Day among moms who had received a
gift from their child (N ¼ 99; ages 38–64, M ¼ 51.9; 11
unspecified). In this study, moms first reported the rela-
tional impact of their gift on a subjective change scale (1 ¼
felt more distant and less connected, 9 ¼ felt closer and
more connected), and then rated the experiential versus
material nature of the gift on a bipolar scale (1 ¼ purely
material, 9 ¼ purely experiential; Van Boven and Gilovich
2003). Like dads, moms who received gifts that were more
experiential reported having a stronger relationship with
their child as a result of receiving the gift (b ¼ 0.21,
SE ¼ 0.07, t(97) ¼ 2.96, p ¼ .004, d ¼ .60). Although these
results are correlational and based on small samples, they
provide preliminary evidence to suggest that experiential
gifts are more effective than material gifts at strengthening
relationships between gift recipients and their gift givers.
RELATIONSHIPS AND THE ROLE OF
EMOTION
Recent experimental research on gift giving has made
great strides in understanding how recipients evaluate dif-
ferent types of gifts (Flynn and Adams 2009; Gino and
Flynn 2011; Waldfogel 1993; Zhang and Epley 2012);
however, less is known about how giver-recipient relation-
ships are best cultivated through different types of gifts
(Aknin and Human 2015). That is, much of the work on
gift giving has focused on how much recipients appreciate,
value, or like particular gifts, rather than the impact of
these gifts on the relationship. For instance, prior gift-
giving experiments have shown that despite gift givers’ be-
liefs that expensive gifts will be more appreciated, recipi-
ents appreciate expensive and inexpensive gifts alike
(Flynn and Adams 2009) and put a lower monetary value
on a gift than its actual cost (Waldfogel 1993). And al-
though gift givers think that unsolicited gifts convey
greater thoughtfulness and serve as a stronger signal of re-
lationship value, recipients prefer receiving cash or gifts
that they had explicitly requested (Gino and Flynn 2011;
Ward and Broniarczyk 2015). Additionally, when buying
for multiple recipients, gift givers select overly
individuated gifts in an attempt to be thoughtful and under-
standing of each unique recipient, but recipients unfortu-
nately tend not to recognize the thought put into gifts they
like (Zhang and Epley 2012), and gift givers’ efforts to
convey thoughtfulness can even result in selecting gifts
that recipients like less (Steffel and LeBoeuf 2014). Even
the most well-intentioned gifts can go awry, as givers also
tend to mispredict how much recipients will appreciate so-
cially responsible gifts, such as charitable donations given
on their behalf (Cavanaugh, Gino, and Fitzsimons 2015).
In light of these findings that gift givers are poor predictors
of what recipients will like, it is fortunate that recipients
2 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
can regift their gifts without offending the giver
(Adams, Flynn, and Norton 2012)!
Our research adopts a different approach to assess a
gift’s value. Namely, we measure the gift’s influence on re-
lationship strength from the recipient’s perspective, rather
than how much the recipient likes the gift. We looked to
the literature on close relationships to define relationship
strength. Although there is substantial variation among re-
lationship types (i.e., friendships, romantic partners, and
family members) with respect to what constitutes a strong
relationship, there are principle indicators of relationship
strength that span across personal relationships: the extent
to which partners feel close to each other (Algoe et al.
2008; Aron, Aron, and Smollan 1992; Dibble, Levine, and
Park 2012; Kok et al. 2013; Kok and Fredrickson 2010)
and connected to each other (Algoe et al. 2008; Dibble
et al. 2012; Hutcherson, Seppala, and Gross 2008), as well
as how satisfied they are with their relationship (Rusbult,
Martz, and Agnew 1998). Across our studies, we adopt
these indicators of relationship strength to measure how
much the gift changes the gift recipient’s perception of the
strength of his or her relationship with the gift giver from
before to after receiving the gift.
This perspective on the success of a gift is similar to that
taken in earlier qualitative research exploring how gift ex-
changes produce relationship change. A series of depth
interviews and surveys offer rich insights into how the con-
text, rituals, meaning, and emotions that surround a gift ex-
change can lead to different relational outcomes ranging
from relationship strengthening to rare cases of relationship
severing (Ruth et al. 1999; Ruth, Brunel, and Otnes 2004).
For instance, Ruth et al. (1999) observed that gift ex-
changes that involve highly personalized rituals can imbue
the gift with shared meaning and often lead to relationship
strengthening. More recently, experimental work has iden-
tified that gifts reflecting the giver can promote relation-
ship closeness (Aknin and Human 2015). The current work
builds on these insights by specifically testing the rela-
tional impact of particular types of gifts—those that are
material versus experiential. It further examines why ex-
periential and material gifts may differ in their ability to
forge a stronger relationship between gift recipients and
givers.
A distinction between experiential and material pur-
chases that has yet to be explored is how much emotion
they evoke during consumption. Prior research has focused
on the happiness elicited by experiences and material pos-
sessions (Van Boven and Gilovich 2003; Weidman and
Dunn 2016), but it is important to note that experiences
can stimulate a wide range of emotions (Bhattacharjee and
Mogilner 2014; Chan et al. 2014; Derbaix and Pham 1991;
Halvena and Holbrook 1986; Mogilner, Aaker, and
Kamvar 2012; Mogilner, Kamvar, and Aaker 2011;
Richins 1997). For instance, a safari adventure can elicit
feelings of awe and fear; a rock concert can fuel
excitement; a spa package can promote calmness; and an
opera may move one to tears. Given the diversity of dis-
crete emotions that consuming both experiences and mater-
ial goods can evoke, we focus our investigation on the
overall intensity of emotion felt during gift consumption,
and propose that the emotion felt by recipients when con-
suming an experiential gift will be more intense than when
consuming a material gift.
Research on relationships highlights emotion as a key
feature in relationship development and maintenance.
Emotions expressed and experienced within the context of
a relationship yield positive interpersonal effects (Clark
and Finkel 2004; Graham et al. 2008; Kubacka et al. 2011;
Laurenceau, Barrett, and Pietromonaco 1998; Slatcher and
Pennebaker 2006). For instance, disclosing one’s emotions
(vs. facts and information) makes people feel closer
(Laurenceau et al. 1998), positive emotions such as grati-
tude promote relationship maintenance behaviors
(Kubacka et al. 2011), and sharing negative emotions pro-
motes bonding (Graham et al. 2008). The relationship-
strengthening effect of emotions extends to situations in
which the emotions are not shared. Prior research has
shown that partners who write about their feelings within a
relationship are
more likely to stay together, even when
they don’t share what they wrote (Slatcher and Pennebaker
2006). More generally, greater emotional intensity has
been found to reduce perceived psychological distance
(Van Boven et al. 2010). Taking these findings together,
we assert that a gift that evokes greater emotion should be
more effective at strengthening relationships than a gift
that elicits a weaker emotional response, and thus experien-
tial gifts should foster stronger relationships than material
gifts. Furthermore, in the case of gifts, the mere fact that
the experience was given by the relationship partner psy-
chologically places the experience and the resulting emo-
tion within the context of the relationship. We therefore
propose that the experiential gift need not be shared be-
tween the gift giver and recipient for it to evoke greater
emotion and thereby improve the relationship.
Notably, the emotion evoked during the gift consump-
tion is distinct from the emotion evoked during the gift ex-
change. In his theoretical model delineating the impact of
gifts on relationships, Sherry (1983) highlights the import-
ance of focusing beyond the gift exchange to the consump-
tion of the gift, during which “the gift becomes the vehicle
by which the relationship of the donor and the recipient is
realigned” (165). Indeed, it is the emotion evoked while
the recipient is consuming the gift that we propose drives
the difference between experiential and material gifts on
relationship change. Whereas material and experiential
gifts are both likely to elicit emotion during a gift exchange
(e.g., a recipient could feel grateful whether given a wallet
or tickets to a comedy show), experiential gifts should
elicit more intense emotion during gift consumption as the
recipient lives through an event (e.g., a recipient may feel
CHAN AND MOGILNER 3
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
mild enjoyment while using a wallet, yet will likely feel in-
tensely amused and delighted while attending a comedy
show; Weidman and Dunn 2016). Additionally, although
Ruth et al. (1999, 2004) found that the valence of the emo-
tion during a gift exchange mattered more than the inten-
sity of emotion in predicting changes in the relationship
(perhaps because the gift giver is likely the source and tar-
get of the emotions evoked during a gift exchange), we
propose that it is the intensity of emotion evoked during
gift consumption that is responsible for the greater power
of experiential gifts to strengthen relationships.
Altogether, we predict that experiential gifts will im-
prove relationships more than material gifts, and that this is
driven by the greater emotional intensity evoked from con-
suming an experience rather than a possession. More for-
mally, we predict:
H1: From the recipient’s perspective, experiential gifts
strengthen relationships more than material gifts, irrespect-
ive of whether the gift is consumed with the gift giver.
H2: Consuming experiential gifts evokes more intense emo-
tion than consuming material gifts, and this greater emotion-
ality drives the effect of gift type on change in relationship
strength.
To test these hypotheses, we conducted experiments
involving actual gift exchanges in the context of existing
personal relationships. In study 1, gift givers were provided
with $15 to purchase either an experiential or material gift
to give to someone they know; in study 2, gift givers were
provided with a coffee mug, framed as experiential or not,
to give to someone they know; and in studies 3 and 4, par-
ticipants recalled experiential or material gifts they had
received from someone they know. Across the studies, the
experiential versus material nature of the gift was manipu-
lated to test how gift type influenced relationship strength
from the recipient’s perspective. To examine the underly-
ing role of emotion, study 3 measured and study 4 manipu-
lated the emotion evoked during gift consumption.
Together, these studies seek to contribute a better under-
standing of how the type of gift received can differentially
affect relationships. Across all four studies, sample size
was determined prior to each study with an effort to collect
as many participants as resources would permit in the allot-
ted timeframe, and all data exclusions and experimental
conditions are reported.
STUDY 1: A $15 GIFT
Study 1 used a two-part design measuring gift recipients’
reports of pre- and post-gift relationship strength to test our
primary hypothesis that experiential gifts strengthen rela-
tionships more than material gifts. Participants were re-
cruited with a friend, and in each participant pair, one was
randomly assigned to be the gift giver and the other to be
the gift recipient. Gift givers were provided with $15 and
instructed to purchase either an experiential gift or material
gift for their friend, which they were not to consume with
their friend. We specifically examined gifts that were not
consumed together to counter the explanation that experi-
ential gifts strengthen relationships solely because sharing
in the experience involves the giver and recipient spending
more time together (Bhattacharjee and Mogilner 2014;
Hershfield, Mogilner, and Barnea 2016; Mogilner and
Aaker 2009).
Method
Fifty-nine pairs of friends (118 participants; 57% fe-
male, 1% unspecified; ages 18–27; M ¼ 20.63) were re-
cruited through a university laboratory to participate in a
gift-giving study in exchange for $10. Upon arriving to the
laboratory, participants in each friend pair were randomly
assigned to the role of gift giver or recipient. Gift givers
were provided with an additional $15 along with instruc-
tions for how to spend this money.
Gift Type Manipulation. Gift givers were randomly as-
signed to purchase either an experiential or material gift
for their friend using definitions adapted from Van Boven
and Gilovich (2003). Gift givers in the experiential gift
condition were instructed, “Purchase a gift that is an ex-
perience ... Experiential gifts are experiences intended for
the recipient to do or live through.” Gift givers in the ma-
terial condition were instructed, “Purchase a gift that is a
material good ... Material gifts are tangible items for the
recipient to have and keep for him/herself.” All gift givers
were further instructed to give a gift that their friend would
consume without them and within the next week, to spend
as close to the $15 as possible on the gift, to give their
friend the gift within the next three days, and not to tell
their friend our instructions regarding the type of gift they
were to purchase. Gift givers left the laboratory with the
$15 and a printout of the gift instructions corresponding to
their assigned condition.
Change in Relationship Strength. To serve as the base-
line measure of relationship strength, gift recipients rated
their relationship with their friend on four items. The first
measure was the inclusion of other in self (IOS) scale
adapted from Aron et al. (1992). Prior research has effect-
ively visually portrayed and measured the sense of inter-
connection central to relationship strength through the
degree of overlap between two circles that represent each
partner’s self-concept (Aknin and Human 2015; Aron et al.
1991; Aron et al. 1992; Brown et al. 2009). We therefore
presented gift recipients with a set of nine circle pairs, in
which one of the circles was labeled “self” and the other
circle was labeled “other.” These pairs ranged in their de-
gree of overlap to represent the strength of the recipient’s
relationship with the gift giver. Gift recipients were asked
4 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
to choose the set of circles that best described their rela-
tionship with their gift giver (see appendix A). Next, re-
cipients were asked to rate their relationship with their
friend on three Likert scales measuring closeness (1 ¼ ex-
tremely distant, 9 ¼ extremely close), connection (1 ¼ ex-
tremely disconnected, 9 ¼ extremely connected), and
relationship strength (1 ¼ extremely weak, 9 ¼ extremely
strong). The average of these four items served as our
measure for pre-gift relationship strength (a ¼ .84).
Recipients were then told that they would be receiving a
gift from their friend within the next three days and a link
to an online follow-up survey from us in one week. They
were instructed to consume the gift they receive once
within the next week (before completing the follow-up sur-
vey), and to not consume the gift with their friend.
Recipients left the laboratory with a printout of their gift
instructions.
One week later, gift recipients received an email inviting
them to complete the online follow-up survey in exchange
for a $5 Amazon.com gift card. Forty-four gift recipients
responded (n
experiential
¼ 20, n
material
¼ 24; 57% female;
ages 18–25, M ¼ 20.5). After describing the gift they had
received, recipients reported the strength of their relation-
ship with their friend using the same four items as before.
These responses were averaged to serve as the post-gift re-
lationship strength measure (a ¼ .93). The post-gift rela-
tionship strength score was subtracted from the pre-gift
score for our measure of change in relationship strength.
One extreme outlier was excluded from further analyses
(greater than three standard deviations from the mean, stu-
dentized residual ¼ 4.72, Cook’s D ¼ 0.59).
Thoughtfulness and Liking. Because much of the ex-
perimental research on gift giving has focused on how
much recipients like the gift and how thoughtful they per-
ceive the gift to be (Flynn and Adams 2009; Gino and
Flynn 2011), we also measured thoughtfulness and liking
to assess whether material and experiential gifts differ on
these dimensions. Recipients rated the thoughtfulness of
their gift on four items adapted from Flynn and Adams
(2009) and Gino and Flynn (2011): the extent to which the
gift was thoughtful, considerate, took their needs into ac-
count, and took what they really wanted into account (1 ¼
not at all, 7 ¼ to a great extent; a ¼ .86). Recipients rated
how much they liked the gift on three items: how much
they liked the gift, how satisfied they were with the gift,
and cost aside, how desirable the gift would be to an aver-
age other person (third item adapted from Rosenzweig
and Gilovich 2012;1¼ not at all, 7 ¼ to a great extent;
a ¼ .85).
Manipulation Checks. As a check for whether gift giv-
ers had followed their instructions, we asked recipients to
1) rate to what extent the gift they received was material or
experiential (1 ¼ purely material, 5 ¼ equally material and
experiential, 9 ¼ purely experiential), 2) report whether
they had shared in the consumption of their gift with their
gift giver, and 3) estimate the price of the gift. Participants
also reported how much time they had spent with their gift
giver during the gift exchange and how much time they
had spent consuming the gift.
Results and Discussion
Gifts Received. Experiential gifts included a pass to a
barre class and movie tickets. Material gifts included a
shirt, a poster, and a wine aerator. The manipulation
check confirmed that recipients in the experiential gift
condition received gifts that were more experiential
(M ¼ 4.89, SD ¼ 2.38) than those in the material gift
condition (M ¼ 3.17, SD ¼ 2.24; t(41) ¼ 2.45, p ¼ .02,
d ¼ .71). Also, the majority of recipients (86%) had not
consumed their gift with their gift giver, there was no
significant difference in estimated price between recipi-
ents of experiential gifts (M ¼ $14.01, SD ¼ 4.19)
and material gifts (M ¼ $13.10, SD ¼ 5.53; t(41) ¼ 0.59,
p ¼ .56, d ¼ .18), and there were no significant differ-
ences in how much time recipients had spent with their
gift giver during the gift exchange (p > .99) or how
much time they had spent consuming their gift (p ¼ .17).
Change in Relationship Strength. The pre-gift relation-
ship measures confirmed that there were no differences in
baseline levels of relationship strength among participants
in the experiential condition (M ¼ 6.71, SD ¼ 2.12) and
material condition (M ¼ 7.10, SD ¼ 2.12; t(41) ¼ 0.95,
p ¼ .35, d ¼ .29). In support of our first hypothesis, the re-
lationship change measure revealed that recipients of an
experiential gift (M ¼ 0.08, SD ¼ 0.79) showed a more
positive change in relationship strength than recipients of a
material gift (M ¼
–0.54, SD ¼ 1.10; t(41) ¼ 2.06, p ¼ .05,
d ¼ .61).
Because som
e participants rated their pre-gift relation-
ship using the extreme ends of the scales, we conducted a
robustness check by trimming the data of any participants
who reported a pre-gift relationship score greater than 8
(n ¼ 11) or less than 2 (n ¼ 0). Omitting these participants
strengthened the effect of gift type on relationship change,
with recipients of experiential gifts reporting greater rela-
tionship improvements than recipients of material gifts
(t(32) ¼ 2.41, p ¼ .02, d ¼ .78).
Thoughtfulness and Liking. Although experiential gifts
were directionally perceived as more thoughtful and better
liked, the effects were not significant (thoughtfulness: M
exp
¼ 5.49, SD ¼ 2.32 vs. M
mat
¼ 5.07, SD ¼ 2.32; t(41) ¼
0.94, p ¼ .35, d ¼ .29; liking: M
exp
¼ 5.68, SD ¼ 0.96 vs.
M
mat
¼ 5.07, SD ¼ 1.43; t(41) ¼ 1.61, p ¼ .12, d ¼ .43).
Therefore, the ability of experiential gifts to strengthen re-
lationships does not appear to be driven by perceived
thoughtfulness or liking.
CHAN AND MOGILNER 5
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
The results of study 1 showed that receiving an experi-
ential gift improves the strength of recipients’ relationships
with their gift giver, compared to receiving a material gift.
Study 2 tests whether highlighting the experiential aspect
of a material gift can similarly improve relationship
strength.
STUDY 2: A COFFEE MUG GIFT
Across a variety of gifts that were individually selected
by each gift giver, study 1 demonstrated the relationship-
strengthening benefits of experiential gifts over material
gifts. This next study provided an even more rigorous test
for the connecting power of experiential gifts by holding
the gift itself constant and varying only the experiential
versus material framing of that gift. Indeed, many material
gifts have experiential components. For example, a stereo
is a material object that is kept in one’s possession for
years, yet it also provides the experience of listening to
music. Similarly, a bottle of wine has a tangible, physical
presence that can contribute to a collection, but it can also
provide a very pleasurable multisensory experience when
consumed with a perfectly paired cheese. Study 2 took ad-
vantage of the malleable distinction between material and
experiential gifts and tested whether a material gift (a cof-
fee mug) could be framed as more experiential (by high-
lighting the experience of drinking coffee) to further
strengthen gift givers’ and recipients’ relationships.
Method
Gift Type Manipulation. Two hundred gift givers were
recruited through a university laboratory (57% female;
ages 18–39, M ¼ 20.6) and provided with a gift-wrapped
coffee mug to give as a gift to someone they know.
Participants were randomly assigned to either give a mug
that highlighted the experience of drinking coffee (with the
words “my coffee time” inscribed on it) or give a mug
identified as a material possession (with the words “my
coffee mug” inscribed on it; see appendix B). A between-
subjects pre-test validated the manipulation: participants
(N ¼ 68; 56% female; ages 18–29, M ¼ 20.94) were pre-
sented with one of the two mugs and asked to rate the mug
on a nine-point scale (1 ¼ purely material, 9 ¼ purely ex-
periential). Participants rated the “my coffee time” mug as
more experiential (M ¼ 3.69, SD ¼ 2.20) than the “my cof-
fee mug” mug (M ¼ 2.63, SD ¼ 1.83; t(67) ¼ 2.13, p ¼
.04, d ¼ .50). The mugs did not differ in rated desirability,
positivity, or favorability (a ¼ .90; t(67) ¼ 0.06, p ¼ .95,
d ¼ .01).
Change in Relationship Strength. A survey link was
provided on a voucher that was inside the gift-wrapped
coffee mug. The voucher was for $5 at a local coffee shop,
and it would become valid if the gift recipient completed a
brief online survey. Each mug condition had a separate sur-
vey link, allowing us to know the type of mug received.
One hundred nine recipients completed the survey (64%
female; ages 16–58, M ¼ 21.5; n
material
¼ 64; n
experiential
¼
45). The survey asked gift recipients to rate how receiving
the gift changed the strength of their relationship with the
person who gave them the gift (1 ¼ felt more disconnected,
9 ¼ felt more connected), and how much they liked the gift
(1 ¼ hate it, 9 ¼ love it).
Results and Discussion
Recipients of the more experiential gift (M ¼ 7.47,
SD ¼ 1.50) reported greater strengthening of their relation-
ship with their gift giver than recipients of the more mater-
ial gift (M ¼ 6.92, SD ¼ 1.34; t(107) ¼ 1.99, p ¼ .05, d ¼
.37). Again, this effect appears to be independent of how
much recipients liked the gift, because recipients reported
no difference in how much they liked their mug (M
exp
¼
7.33, SD ¼ 1.41 vs. M
mat
¼ 7.25, SD ¼ 1.50; t(107) ¼
0.29, p ¼ .77, d ¼ .06).
Study 2 provided additional support for our main hy-
pothesis (hypothesis 1) using a highly conservative and
controlled test for the effect of gift type on change in rela-
tionship strength. Holding all features of the gift constant
except for the extent to which the giving of an experience
was highlighted, this study showed that receiving a more
experiential gift is better at strengthening relationships
than receiving a more material gift. Indeed, even a material
gift (a coffee mug) could be made more connecting by re-
minding the recipient of the experience it offers (the time
spent drinking coffee). Many gifts have both experiential
and material elements, and these results demonstrate that
gift givers can enjoy
AQ1
some of the relational benefit of ex-
periential gifts by merely highlighting the experience the
gift provides. The next study explored a mechanism for the
effect, testing the underlying role of emotion from gift
consumption.
STUDY 3: THE MEDIATING ROLE OF
EMOTION FROM GIFT CONSUMPTION
Studies 1 and 2 provided evidence for experiential gifts
being more effective at strengthening relationships than
material gifts. In this next study, we explored the underly-
ing role of emotion. Participants in study 3 were asked to
recall either an experiential or material gift they had
received and then to rate how the gift impacted their rela-
tionship with the gift giver. To examine the mechanism,
this study additionally measured the intensity of the emo-
tion evoked from gift consumption separately from the
emotion evoked from the gift exchange. Qualitative re-
search observed that a gift exchange can be highly emo-
tional, and the combination of negative and positive
emotions felt during a gift exchange, as well as the
6 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
recipient’s reaction to the emotions expressed by the gift
giver, contribute to relationship change (Belk and Coon
1993; Ruth et al. 1999, 2004). We predict that while a gift
exchange can be highly emotional for both material and ex-
periential gifts, consuming an experiential gift will elicit a
more intense emotional response than consuming a mater-
ial gift (Weidman and Dunn 2016). For example, attend-
ing a theatre performance or going on a vacation is likely
to be more emotional than wearing a new pair of boots or
driving a car. Furthermore, it is this emotion evoked from
consuming experiential gifts that we propose is respon-
sible for their positive impact on relationship strength
(hypothesis 2).
Though study 1 showed the more positive effect of
receiving an experiential gift versus a material gift even
when participants were instructed to not consume the gift
together, this study further examined the role of sharing the
gift through a 2 (gift type: material vs. experiential) 2
(consumption: shared vs. nonshared) between-subjects
design.
Method
Gift Type Manipulation. Six hundred adults were re-
cruited through Amazon Mechanical Turk to participate in
this study in exchange for $0.75. Participants were ran-
domly assigned to recall a particular type of gift they had
received: shared experiential gift, nonshared experiential
gift, shared material gift, or nonshared material gift.
Participants in the experiential gift conditions were in-
structed, “Please recall and describe an experiential gift
that you have received at some point in your life from an-
other person.” Participants in the material gift conditions
were instructed, “Please recall and describe a material gift
that you have received at some point in your life from an-
other person.” Participants were provided with a definition
of material or experiential gifts adapted from Van Boven
and Gilovich (2003). Those in the shared consumption con-
ditions were further instructed, “This should be [a material/
an experiential] gift that you consumed with the person
who gave it to you (i.e., you shared the gift with your gift
giver).” Those in the nonshared consumption conditions
were further instructed, “This should be [a material/an ex-
periential] gift that you consumed on your own (i.e., you
did not share the gift with your gift giver).”
Participants who could not recall a gift (n ¼ 7), did not
complete the survey (n ¼ 41), or did not follow the gift re-
call instructions (i.e., described a gift they had given, n ¼
1; described a gift received from multiple people, n ¼ 15;
described a gift of cash, n ¼ 1; described multiple gifts, n
¼ 1) were eliminated from the analysis. This left 534 gift
recipients in the analyzed dataset (59% female; ages 18–
78, M ¼ 33.1).
Change in Relationship Strength. Using the measures
from study 1, participants rated the strength of their rela-
tionship to the gift giver before and after receiving the gift.
Participants first chose two pairs of overlapping circles:
one to represent their relationship before receiving the gift
and one to represent their relationship after receiving the
gift (see appendix A; adapted from Aron et al. 1992).
Participants also rated their relationship both before (a ¼
.92) and after (a ¼ .91) receiving the gift in terms of close-
ness (1 ¼ extremely distant, 9 ¼ extremely close), connec-
tion (1 ¼ extremely disconnected, 9 ¼ extremely
connected), and relationship strength (1 ¼ extremely weak,
9 ¼ extremely strong). The difference between each of the
before and after ratings on the four relationship measures
was calculated, and these values were averaged to form an
overall indicator of change in relationship strength.
Emotion. Recipients reported how emotional they felt
from the gift exchange separately from how emotional they
felt during gift consumption. They were specifically in-
structed, “Think about the emotions you felt from receiving
the gift. Focus on the moment when you felt the most emo-
tional from receiving the gift, and rate how intensely you
felt that emotion” (1 ¼ did not feel emotional at all from
receiving the gift, 7 ¼ felt extremely emotional from
receiving the gift); and “Think about the emotions you felt
from consuming the gift. Focus on the moment when you
felt the most emotional from consuming the gift, and rate
how intensely you felt that emotion” (1 ¼ did not feel emo-
tional at all from consuming the gift, 7 ¼
felt extremely
emotional from
consuming the gift). We asked participants
to focus on the moment they felt most emotional to remove
the influence of hedonic adaptation that is more likely to
have occurred for the more durable material gifts (Nicolao
et al. 2009). To account for this difference in durability, we
also asked participants to estimate the total amount of time
they had spent consuming the gift.
To explore the specific emotions evoked by their gifts,
we then asked participants to select one primary emotion
from a list of 30 randomly ordered discrete emotions that
they were feeling at the moment they felt most emotional
(see appendix C). This list was followed by a text box, in
case the emotion they felt was not provided. The listed
emotions were drawn from the Positive and Negative
Affective Schedule—Expanded Form (PANAS-X; Watson
and Clark 1994), including the two general dimension
scales (10 positive and 10 negative emotions), along with
eight additional basic emotions (four positive and four
negative). Given our interest in the social aspects of a gift
exchange and consumption, we also added two emotions
(embarrassed and grateful) that serve important social
functions (Fischer and Manstead 2008; Tooby and
Cosmides 2008).
CHAN AND MOGILNER 7
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Thoughtfulness and Liking. Thoughtfulness and liking
of the gift were measured using the same items as in study
1. Perceived thoughtfulness was measured using four
items (a ¼ .84), and liking was measured using three items
(a ¼ .73).
Other Features of the Gift. To account for the likely
variation among the gifts received, we asked recipients to
estimate the price of the gift, to report when they had
received the gift, and to indicate how they were related to
their gift giver (spouse or significant other, child or grand-
child, parent, another family member, close friend, ac-
quaintance, colleague, or other).
Lastly, participants responded to manipulation checks
by rating the extent to which the gift they received was ma-
terial or experiential (1 ¼ purely material, 5 ¼ equally ma-
terial and experiential, 9 ¼ purely experiential), and by
indicating whether they had consumed the gift with their
gift giver (yes, no).
Results
Gifts Received. Shared experiential gifts included vac-
ations, meals, and tickets to concerts or sporting events.
Nonshared experiential gifts included music or dance les-
sons, spa services, vacations, meals, and tickets for events
that were not attended with the gift giver. Shared material
gifts included coffee makers, game consoles, televisions,
tablet computers, and cars; and nonshared material gifts
included jewelry, clothing, computers, portable music
players, and digital cameras. Manipulation checks con-
firmed that participants in the experiential gift conditions
recalled gifts that were more experiential (M ¼ 7.55,
SE ¼ 0.13) than participants in the material gift conditions
(M ¼ 2.90, SE ¼ 0.13; t(532) ¼ 25.49, p < .001, d ¼ 1.48);
and most participants in the shared gift conditions (93%)
consumed their gifts with their gift giver (vs. 3% in the
nonshared gift conditions; v
2
(1) ¼ 435.96, p < .001).
Participants in the experiential gift conditions also con-
sumed their gift over a shorter period of time (M ¼ 3.41
days, SE ¼ 12.56) than participants in the material gift con-
ditions (M ¼ 118.98 days, SE ¼ 12.24; t(532) ¼ 6.59, p <
.001, d ¼ .55), consistent with the more durable nature of
material gifts.
The estimated price of the gifts ranged from $1 (a mag-
net) to $19,000 (a car). The majority of gifts (60%) were
received within the past year, but the oldest gift was
received in 1969. Most gifts were received from a spouse
or significant other (37%), parent (19%), another family
member (16%), or a close friend (19%).
Change in Relationship Strength. A2 2 ANOVA
conducted on change in relationship strength revealed only
a main effect of gift type, with experiential gifts (M ¼
0.66, SE ¼ 0.05) strengthening relationships more than
material gifts (M ¼ 0.40, SE ¼ 0.05; F(1, 530) ¼ 11.81,
p < .001, d ¼ .30; see figure 1). Neither the shared con-
sumption main effect (p ¼ .50) nor the gift type by shared
consumption interaction (p ¼ .81) was significant, suggest-
ing that the relationship-strengthening effect of receiving
an experiential gift occurred regardless of whether recipi-
ents consumed the gift with their gift giver (see web
appendix A for robustness check).
Given the wide range of gifts, we also conducted a 2
(gift type) 2 (shared) ANCOVA on change in relation-
ship strength, controlling for estimated price, date of re-
ceipt, and how the recipient was related to the gift giver
(dummy coded). Results again showed that receiving an
experiential gift strengthened relationships more than
receiving a material gift (F(1, 520) ¼ 6.83, p ¼ .009, d ¼
.23). Including the covariates did not affect the significance
levels of the shared consumption main effect (p ¼ .72) or
the interaction effect (p ¼ .32).
Emotion from Consumption. To examine the role of
emotion, we first conducted a 2 (gift type) 2 (shared)
ANOVA
on the extent to which consuming the gift made
recipients feel emotional. The results revealed only a main
effect of gift type, with experiential gifts (M ¼ 5.14,
SE ¼ 0.09) evoking greater emotion than material gifts (M
¼ 4.70, SE ¼ 0.09; F(1, 530) ¼ 11.08, p < .001, d ¼ .29).
There was a nonsignificant effect of sharing (p ¼ .14) and
a nonsignificant interaction effect (p ¼ .50).
These effects held when the covariates were included in
the analysis. The results again revealed only a main effect
of gift type, with experiential gifts evoking greater emotion
than material gifts (F(1, 520) ¼ 15.55, p < .001, d ¼ .34),
and a nonsignificant effect of sharing (p ¼ .92) and inter-
action effect (p ¼ .90). Accounting for factors such as the
type of relationship and the time that has passed since the
gift exchange, these results suggest that consuming an ex-
periential gift evokes greater emotion than consuming a
material gift, regardless of whether recipients consume the
gift with their gift giver. The vast majority of the specific
FIGURE 1
STUDY 3: RELATIONSHIPS IMPROVED MORE AMONG
RECIPIENTS OF EXPERIENTIAL (VS. MATERIAL) GIFTS
0
0.2
0.4
0.6
0.8
1
Material Experiential
Relationship change
Nonshared Shared
8 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
emotions participants felt most intensely while consuming
the gift were positive (97.6%; see table 1).
We next conducted a mediation analysis to test our the-
ory that the positive effect of receiving an experiential gift
(vs. material gift) on relationship strength is driven by the
greater emotion evoked during gift consumption. We
entered recipients’ ratings of how emotional consuming
the gift was as the mediator, controlling for estimated
price, date of receipt, and how the recipient was related to
the gift giver. As our previous analyses showed, experien-
tial gifts strengthened relationships more than material
gifts (b ¼ 0.10, SE ¼ 0.04, t(522) ¼ 2.70, p ¼ .007, d ¼
.23). In addition, gifts that were more emotional were more
effective at improving relationship strength (b ¼ 0.14,
SE ¼ 0.02, t(522) ¼ 33.95, p < .001, d ¼ .49). When both
gift type and emotion were entered into the model predict-
ing change in relationship strength, the effect of consump-
tion emotion remained significant (b ¼ 0.13, SE ¼ 0.02,
t(521) ¼ 5.44, p < .001, d ¼ .46), whereas the effect of
gift type was no longer significant (b ¼ 0.07, SE ¼ 0.04,
t(521) ¼ 1.80, p ¼ .07, d ¼ .15). Corroborating evidence
was obtained in a bootstrap analysis, which generated a
confidence interval of the indirect effect that did not cross
zero (95% CI ¼ [.03, .12]; Hayes 2012; Zhao, Lynch, and
Chen 2010; see figure 2). In sum, experiential gifts tend to
be more emotional to consume, and gifts that are more
emotional to consume lead recipients to have a stronger re-
lationship with their gift giver, thus supporting our hypoth-
esis that experiential gifts strengthen relationships more
than material gifts because they evoke greater emotional
intensity during consumption (hypothesis 2).
Emotion from Gift Exchange. Having identified the
significant role of the emotion evoked during gift con-
sumption, we next examined the emotion evoked during
the gift exchange. A 2 2 ANOVA conducted on how
emotional recipients felt upon receiving the gift showed a
nonsignificant main effect of gift type. Material and ex-
periential gifts did not differ in how emotional it was to re-
ceive the gift (p ¼ .88). The main effect of shared
consumption (p > .99) and
the gift type shared inter-
action (p ¼ .63) were also not significant. Like the emo-
tions evoked during gift consumption, the vast majority of
the specific emotions participants felt most during the gift
exchange were positive (96.8%; see table 1). These find-
ings are consistent with our theorizing that experiential and
material gifts are similarly emotional when received, and
thus it is the emotion felt from gift consumption, rather
than the gift exchange, that is responsible for the greater re-
lationship-strengthening effect of experiential gifts.
TABLE 1
STUDY 2: EMOTIONS FELT MOST INTENSELY
DURING GIFT CONSUMPTION AND GIFT RECEIPT
(FIVE MOST COMMONLY REPORTED)
Gift consumption Gift receipt
Emotion
%of
participants
Emotion
%of
participants
Happy 29.0% Grateful 20.0%
Delighted/cheerful 15.9% Delighted/cheerful 17.6%
Grateful 13.1% Excited 17.4%
Excited 10.3% Happy 13.7%
Enthusiastic 6.9% Surprised 13.5%
FIGURE 2
AQ6
STUDY 3: EXPERIENTIAL GIFTS WERE MORE EMOTIONAL TO CONSUME AND THEREFORE MORE CONNECTING
Note: ** p < .01, *** p < .001, two-tailed test. Parameter estimates are listed with standard
errors in parentheses, with estimated price of gift, date of gift receipt, and how the recipient
was related to the
g
ift
g
iver
(
dumm
y
coded
)
as model covariates.
Experiential gift
vs.
material gift
Emotion from gift
consumption
Relationship
change
a = .26(.07)*** b = .13(.02)***
c = .10(.04)**
c' = .07(.04)
CHAN AND MOGILNER 9
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Thoughtfulness and Liking. We again found no differ-
ences in thoughtfulness and liking across conditions. A 2
2 ANOVA predicting perceived thoughtfulness of the gift
revealed no significant effects for gift type (p ¼ .77),
shared consumption (p ¼ .16), or the interaction (p ¼ .08).
Similarly, a 2 2 ANOVA predicting liking of the gift re-
vealed no significant effects for gift type (p ¼ .19), shared
consumption (p ¼ .42), or the interaction (p ¼ .75).
Discussion
Examining a wide range of real-world gifts across a var-
iety of relationships, this study provided robust evidence
that experiential gifts strengthen relationships more than
material gifts, regardless of whether gift recipients and giv-
ers consume the gift together (hypothesis 1). Furthermore,
the mechanism underlying this effect is the intensity of
emotion evoked during gift consumption, which is distinct
from the emotion evoked during the gift exchange.
Specifically, consuming experiential gifts evokes greater
emotion than consuming material gifts, and it is this emo-
tional intensity that strengthens recipients’ relationships
with their gift givers (hypothesis 2).
Because the vast majority of participants in study 3 re-
ported the emotion they felt most intensely while consum-
ing their gift to be positive, there was not sufficient data to
assess whether the effect of emotion on relationship
strength would generalize to negative emotions felt during
gift consumption. For example, would an intense feeling of
sadness while watching a performance of Madame
Butterfly or an intense feeling of fear while watching
Silence of the Lambs strengthen the giver-recipient rela-
tionship? To explore the role of emotional valence, we
conducted a similar study in which we asked participants
(N ¼ 523; 46% female, three unspecified; ages 18–66, M
¼ 32.0, one unspecified) to recall a significant material or
experiential gift they had received. Participants rated how
much their relationship with the gift giver had strengthened
as a result of the gift, as well as how intensely they felt
each of 30 discrete emotions while consuming their gift
(15 positive emotions and 15 negative emotions; see ap-
pendix C). Ratings for all 30 emotions were averaged to
create an index of overall emotion. In addition, the ratings
for the positive and the negative emotions were averaged
separately. The results showed that recipients of experien-
tial (vs. material) gifts felt more emotional overall (M
exp
¼
3.29, SE ¼ 0.07 vs. M
mat
¼ 3.02, SE ¼ 0.07; F(1, 510) ¼
20.02, p < .001, d ¼ .39), and this effect held for purely
positive emotions (M
exp
¼ 3.73, SE ¼ 0.06 vs. M
mat
¼
3.52, SE ¼ 0.07; F (1, 510) ¼ 12.96, p < .001, d ¼ .30),
and purely negative emotions (M
exp
¼ 2.22, SE ¼ 0.07 vs.
M
mat
¼ 2.08, SE ¼ 0.07; F(1, 510) ¼ 5.09, p ¼ .03, d ¼
.20). Furthermore, significant indirect effects were
observed when we used the average of all 30 discrete emo-
tions (95% CI ¼ [.05, .15]), as well as just the 15 positive
emotions (95% CI ¼ [.04, .14]), and just the 15 negative
emotions (95% CI ¼ [.003, .08]) as mediators for the effect
of gift type on change in relationship strength. This offers
preliminary evidence suggesting that strong negative emo-
tions evoked through gift consumption can also strengthen
giver-recipient relationships.
STUDY 4: THE MODERATING ROLE OF
EMOTION FROM GIFT CONSUMPTION
Building on the mediation evidence from study 3, study
4 sought further evidence for the underlying role of emo-
tion from gift consumption through a test of moderation.
This study followed a 2 (gift type: material vs. experien-
tial) 2 (emotion: control vs. emotion) between-subjects
design. Participants were asked to recall either an experien-
tial gift or material gift they had received, and half were
more specifically instructed to recall a gift that had evoked
intense emotion during consumption.
Additionally, this study design allowed us to identify a
boundary condition for the benefit of receiving experiential
gifts over material gifts. Although we argue that consum-
ing experiential gifts tends to evoke greater emotion than
consuming material gifts, there surely are some material
gifts that elicit a great deal of emotion when consumed.
For example, wearing one’s engagement ring hopefully
makes one feel incredibly loved and loving, and looking at
a photograph that captures a meaningful moment should
stir emotion. Because we argue that experiential gifts
strengthen giver-recipient relationships by eliciting more
intense emotion during consumption, material gifts that
evoke intense emotion should similarly strengthen
relationships.
Method
One thousand forty-two participants were recruited
through Amazon Mechanical Turk to participate in this
study in exchange for $0.50. Participants who did not com-
plete the survey (n ¼ 26) or wrote that they could not fol-
low the instructions or could not think of a gift (n ¼ 21)
were eliminated from the analysis. This left 995 partici-
pants in the analyzed dataset (45% female, two unspeci-
fied; ages 18–77, M ¼ 33.2).
Gift Manipulations. Participants were randomly as-
signed to one of the four conditions comprising the 2 (gift
type: material vs. experiential) 2 (emotion: control vs.
emotion) design. Participants in the control conditions
were asked to “Please recall and describe [a material/an ex-
periential] gift you’ve received.” Participants in the emo-
tion conditions were asked to “Please recall and describe [a
material/an experiential] gift you’ve received that makes or
made you feel emotional while consuming it.” Participants
were provided with the definition of material or
10 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
experiential gifts from studies 1 and 3. All participants
were also instructed that the gift should be one they
received from someone they know and one they did not
consume with the gift giver.
Change in Relationship Strength. This study used yet
another measure for change in relationship strength, which
was adapted from the Relationship Satisfaction Level
index of Rusbult et al.’s (1998) Investment Model Scale.
Participants rated their agreement (1 ¼ don’t agree at all, 5
¼ somewhat agree, 9 ¼ agree completely) with five state-
ments (a ¼ .95): “I feel more satisfied with our relation-
ship as a result of the gift”; “Our relationship is closer to
ideal as a result of the gift”; “Our relationship is much bet-
ter than others’ relationships as a result of the gift”; “Our
relationship makes me very happy as a result of the gift”;
and “Our relationship does a good job of fulfilling my
needs for intimacy, companionship, etc. as a result of the
gift.”
Thoughtfulness and Liking. Thoughtfulness and liking
of the gift were measured using the same items as in stud-
ies 1 and 3. Again, perceived thoughtfulness of the gift was
measured using four items (a ¼ .78), and liking was meas-
ured using three items (a ¼ .56).
Other Features of the Gift. To account for the variation
among the gifts received, we asked recipients to estimate
the price of the gift, to report when they had received the
gift, and to indicate how they were related to their gift
giver (spouse or significant other, child or grandchild, par-
ent, another family member, close friend, acquaintance,
colleague, or other) and for what occasion they had
received the gift (birthday, wedding, anniversary,
Christmas, Valentine’s Day, Mother’s Day, Father’s Day,
graduation, for no special occasion, or other).
Lastly, participants responded to manipulation checks
by rating the extent to which the gift they received was ma-
terial or experiential (1 ¼ purely material, 5 ¼ equally ma-
terial and experiential, 9 ¼ purely experiential), and how
emotional they had felt while consuming the gift (1 ¼ did
not feel emotional at all from consuming the gift, 9 ¼ felt
extremely emotional from consuming the gift).
Results
Gifts Received. As in study 3, the experiential gifts re-
called included vacation, meals, and tickets to perform-
ances and events. The recalled material gifts included
clothing, electronics, musical instruments, and jewelry.
A2 2 ANOVA conducted on the material-experiential
manipulation check confirmed a main effect of gift type
(F(1, 991) ¼ 1157.74, p < .001, d ¼ 1.45), along with
a main effect of emotion condition (F(1, 991) ¼ 11.72,
p < .001, d ¼ .15) and an interaction (F(1, 991) ¼ 34.88,
p < .001, d ¼ .25). The experiential gifts were rated as
more experiential than the material gifts in the control
conditions (M
exp
¼ 7.72, SE ¼ .13 vs. M
mat
¼ 2.42, SE ¼
.13; t(991) ¼ 28.85, p < .001, d ¼ 1.70) and emotion con-
ditions (M
exp
¼ 7.39, SE ¼ .13 vs. M
mat
¼ 3.66, SE ¼ .14;
t(991) ¼ 19.48, p < .001, d ¼ 1.19).
A2 2 ANOVA conducted on the emotion manipula-
tion check confirmed a main effect of emotion condition
(F(1, 991) ¼ 105.91, p < .001, d ¼ .62), as well as a main
effect of gift type (F(1, 991) ¼ 11.83, p < .001, d ¼ .21),
and an interaction (F(1, 991) ¼ 12.84, p < .001, d ¼ .22).
The recalled emotional gifts were rated as more emotional
than the control gifts in the experiential conditions (M
emot
¼ 5.63, SE ¼ .09 vs. M
cont
¼ 5.02, SE ¼ .09; t(991) ¼
4.84, p < .001, d ¼ .40) and material conditions (M
emot
¼
5.64, SE ¼ .10 vs. M
cont
¼ 4.38, SE ¼ .09; t(991) ¼ 9.62,
p < .001, d ¼ .83). Consistent with our theorizing that ex-
periential gifts tend to evoke more intense emotion during
consumption than material gifts, these results show that in
the control conditions, experiential gifts were more emo-
tional than the material gifts (M
exp
¼ 5.02, SE ¼ .09 vs.
M
mat
¼ 4.38, SE ¼ .09; t(991) ¼ 5.07, p < .001, d ¼ .42).
Employing yet another approach to check the emotion
manipulation, we conducted a textual analysis on partici-
pants’ written description of their gift using the Linguistic
Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC; Pennebaker, Booth, and
Francis 2007), which is an effective measure of the amount
of emotion expressed (Kahn et al. 2007). The LIWC enum-
erated the percentage of emotion words written by each
participant. This additional check confirmed that, overall,
participants in the emotion conditions expressed greater
emotion when writing about their gift, and that within the
control conditions, participants in the experiential condi-
tion also expressed greater emotion when writing about
their gift. Specifically, participants in the experiential emo-
tion (M ¼ 6.89, SE ¼ 0.31), material emotion (M ¼ 6.42,
SE ¼ 0.33), and experiential control (M ¼ 5.08, SE ¼ 0.31)
conditions each wrote a significantly higher percentage of
emotion words than did participants in the material control
condition (M ¼ 3.83, SE ¼ 0.31; p’s < .005). Again, this is
consistent with our theorizing that experiential gifts tend to
evoke greater emotion than material gifts.
The estimated price of the gifts ranged from $0 (e.g., a
handed-down shirt) to $25,000 (a wedding ring). Fifty per-
cent of gifts were received within the past year, and the
oldest gift was received in 1960. Most gifts were received
from a spouse or significant other (32%), parent (24%), an-
other family member (17%), or a close friend (18%), and
were received for a birthday (34%), Christmas (25%), or
no special occasion (26%). Wedding and anniversary gifts
combined constituted less than 6% of the gifts.
Change in Relationship Strength. A 2 (gift type) 2
(emotion) ANOVA conducted on change in relationship
strength revealed a main effect of gift type (F(1, 991) ¼
21.33, p < .001, d ¼ .29), a main effect of emotion
(F(1, 991) ¼ 17.81, p < .001, d ¼ .26), and the predicted
CHAN AND MOGILNER 11
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
interaction (F(1, 991) ¼ 10.62, p ¼ .001, d ¼ .21).
Consistent with our previous findings, in the control condi-
tions, experiential gifts (M ¼ 5.78, SE ¼ 0.13) resulted in
greater improvements in relationship strength than material
gifts (M ¼ 4.71, SE ¼ 0.13; t(991) ¼ 5.69, p < .001, d ¼
.49). Furthermore, in support of our proposed underlying
role of emotion, material gifts that evoked intense emotion
during consumption (M ¼ 5.73, SE ¼ 0.15) resulted in
similar improvements in relationship strength as experien-
tial gifts that evoked emotion (M ¼ 5.92, SE ¼ 0.13; t(991)
¼ 0.94, p ¼ .35, d ¼ .08; figure 3). Controlling for the esti-
mated price of the gift, date of receipt, how the recipient
was related to the gift giver (dummy coded), and the gift
occasion (dummy coded) did not affect the significance of
the main effect of gift type (F(1, 972) ¼ 15.72, p < .001,
d ¼ .26), emotion (F(1, 972) ¼ 15.20, p < .001, d ¼ .24),
or their interaction (F(1, 972) ¼ 10.10, p ¼ .002, d ¼ .20).
We observed that many of the emotional material gifts
were pieces of jewelry commemorating a meaningful life
event (e.g., engagement or wedding, birth of a child, gradu-
ation) or passed down as heirlooms. Others included photo-
graphs and religious items (e.g., bible, rosary). Though the
predicted effects of gift type and emotion on change in re-
lationship held when we controlled for gift occasion and
other features of the gifts (i.e., price, date of receipt, and re-
lationship to the gift giver), it is still possible that the emo-
tion manipulation elicited gifts in participants’ minds that
differed in ways other than their material or experiential
distinction. To assess this, we asked two research assistants
who were blind to study conditions and hypotheses to cat-
egorize the gifts participants had received. First, the re-
search assistants jointly determined purchase categories
that encompassed the full range of gifts (e.g., travel; food
and drink; clothing, shoes, and accessories). The research
assistants then independently assigned each gift to one of
the 10 purchase categories (84% agreement, and disagree-
ments were resolved through discussion). An examination
of the coded gifts revealed some differences among the
material gifts between the control and emotion conditions
(see table 2). For example, jewelry was more likely to be
mentioned as an emotional material gift, and though elec-
tronics were also frequently mentioned as an emotional
material gift, they were more likely to be mentioned in the
control condition. More importantly, however, the pur-
chase category of the gifts received did not affect the pri-
mary outcome of relationship change. Controlling for
purchase category (dummy coded), a 2 (gift type) 2
(emotion) ANCOVA conducted on change in relationship
strength still revealed a main effect of gift type (F(1, 959)
¼ 3.89, p < .05, d ¼ .20), a main effect of emotion
FIGURE 3
STUDY 4: RELATIONSHIPS IMPROVED MORE AMONG
RECIPIENTS WHO RECEIVED EXPERIENTIAL (VS. MATERIAL)
GIFTS OR GIFTS THAT WERE EMOTIONAL TO CONSUME
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
Control Emotion
Relationship change
Material Experiential
TABLE 2
STUDY 4: PERCENTAGE OF GIFTS RECEIVED (BY CONDITION) AND MEAN CHANGE IN RELATIONSHIP STRENGTH IN EACH
PURCHASE CATEGORY
Material Experiential Mean change in
relationship strength
Control Emotion Control Emotion
(n ¼ 259) (n ¼ 220) (n ¼ 258) (n ¼ 258)
Travel 0% 0.9% 24.4%* 16.3%* 6.18
Recreation and leisure 1.5% 2.7% 21.3% 16.7% 6.10
Food and drink 0%* 2.3%* 19.4% 19.4% 5.67
Beauty and wellness 2.7% 2.3% 8.1% 6.2% 5.60
Arts, music, and literature 5.8% 9.6% 20.9% 25.2% 5.58
Jewelry 10.8%* 19.6%* 0% 1.6% 5.54
Electronics and technology 36.7%* 19.6%* 0%* 4.3%* 5.27
Home and garden 8.1% 13.2% 0% 0.8% 5.21
Other 7.0%* 12.7%* 3.9% 5.4% 5.19
Clothing, shoes, and accessories 27.4%* 17.3%* 1.2% 4.3% 4.86
Note: Within each material or experiential gift condition, * represents a statistical difference between the control and emotion conditions for each purchase
category at p < .05.
12 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
(F(1, 959) ¼ 19.39, p < .001, d ¼ .28), and the predicted
interaction (F(1, 959) ¼ 8.42, p ¼ .004, d ¼ .18); the effect
of purchase category was not significant (F(1, 959) ¼ 1.43,
p ¼ .17, d ¼ .22), and none of the individual purchase cate-
gories significantly predicted the change in relationship
strength. This suggests that the specific purchases gener-
ated in each condition cannot explain the effect of con-
sumption emotion on relationship strength for experiential
versus material gifts. Moreover, an examination of the
average change in relationship strength across the various
purchase categories reflects a pattern consistent with the
primary hypothesis: recipients of gifts in more experiential
categories reported greater improvements in relationship
strength than those who received gifts in more material cat-
egories (see table 2).
Thoughtfulness and Liking. A2 2 ANOVA predict-
ing thoughtfulness revealed only a main effect of emotion,
with emotional gifts (M ¼ 6.23, SE ¼ .04) being con-
sidered more thoughtful than control gifts (M ¼ 6.12,
SE ¼ 0.04; F(1, 991) ¼ 3.85, p ¼ .05, d ¼ .12). The effects
of gift type and the interaction were not significant (p’s >
.16). A 2 2 ANOVA predicting liking revealed a main
effect for gift type, with experiential gifts (M ¼ 6.18, SE ¼
.04) being better liked than material gifts (M ¼ 6.04,
SE ¼ 0.04; F(1, 991) ¼ 6.31, p ¼ .01, d ¼ .16). The effects
of the emotion manipulation and the interaction were not
significant (p
’s > .50). When thoughtfulness and liking
were included as covariates in the analysis of change in re-
lationship strength, the results held: we still observed a
main effect of gift type (F(1, 989) ¼ 19.71, p < .001, d ¼
.27), of emotion (F(1, 989) ¼ 15.68, p < .001, d ¼ .24),
and their interaction (F(1, 989) ¼ 10.20, p < .001, d ¼
.19). Again, this suggests that perceived thoughtfulness
and liking of the gift are not responsible for the greater
ability of experiential gifts to strengthen relationships.
Discussion
Using yet another measure of change in relationship
strength, the control conditions in study 4 replicated the
beneficial effect of receiving experiential gifts over mater-
ial gifts observed in the previous studies. This effect was
robust even when we controlled for many other features of
the gift. Moreover, this study used a test of moderation to
provide additional evidence for the underlying role of con-
sumption emotion and to identify an important boundary
condition for the effect: material gifts that made recipients
feel emotional while consuming them were as effective at
strengthening the relationship as experiential gifts.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
Consumers spend a lot of money on others (Americans
spend approximately $130 billion on gifts per year; Unity
Marketing 2015), and spending money on others has been
proven to increase one’s own happiness (Dunn, Aknin, and
Norton 2008a). The current research explores the more far-
reaching effect on relationships between people, finding
that not all prosocial expenditures are equally beneficial.
Despite gift givers’ tendencies to give material posses-
sions, our findings show that material gifts are less effect-
ive than experiential gifts at strengthening
AQ2
relationships
between gift givers and their recipients.
This research provides guidance for gift givers on what
to give and offers insight into the relational function of
gifts. Taking the recipients’ perspective to assess the suc-
cess of gifts, we conducted experiments involving a variety
of real-life gift exchanges and ways of measuring relation-
ship change, and we consistently found that experiential
gifts strengthen relationships more than material gifts
(studies 1–4). This effect also emerged when the very same
gift was framed as being relatively more experiential
(study 2). A driving factor underlying this effect is the
greater level of emotion elicited when recipients consume
experiential gifts versus material gifts, which we identified
through tests of mediation (study 3) and moderation (study
4). Even though there was no difference in the intensity of
emotion recipients felt upon receiving experiential and ma-
terial gifts, recipients felt more emotional when consuming
experiential (vs. material) gifts, which served to strengthen
their relationship with the gift giver. From this, we learn
that gift givers seeking to foster closer relationships with
their recipients are likely to achieve greater success by giv-
ing experiential gifts, rather than material gifts.
Theoretical Contributions
To build on the now-established body of work that has
demonstrated that purchasing experiences (vs. material
goods) for oneself positively affects one’s personal well-
being (Carter and Gilovich 2010; Gilovich et al. 2015;
Nicolao et al. 2009; Rosenzweig and Gilovich 2012; Van
Boven and Gilovich 2003), research has begun investigat-
ing factors that precede experiential and material purchas-
ing (Dai, Chan, and Mogilner 2016; Kumar and Gilovich
2016; Kumar, Killingsworth, and Gilovich 2014; Tully,
Hershfield, and Meyvis 2015). Findings suggest that the
benefit of acquiring experiences for the purchaser can be
largely explained by the typically more social nature of ex-
periences (Bhattacharjee and Mogilner 2014; Caprariello
and Reis 2013). Our findings further contribute to this bur-
geoning stream of research by being the first to show the
interpersonal consequences of experiential versus material
purchases. In addition, we identify a novel advantage of
experiential purchases: consuming an experience evokes
greater emotion than consuming a material possession.
Our finding that the emotion felt during gift consump-
tion is responsible for strengthening relationships is con-
sistent with past work on interpersonal relationships that
CHAN AND MOGILNER 13
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
has highlighted the importance of emotion in close rela-
tionships (Aron et al. 2000; Bazzini et al. 2007; Clark and
Finkel 2004; Laurenceau et al. 1998; Nummenmaa et al.
2012; Peters and Kashima 2007; Raghunathan and
Corfman 2006; Ramanathan and McGill 2007; Slatcher
and Pennebaker 2006). Our research builds on this litera-
ture by showing that the gift of an emotional experience
can strengthen relationships, even when relationship part-
ners do not consume the gift together.
Our research also contributes to gift-giving research by
testing how different types of gifts impact relationships
and by examining the emotion evoked from gift consump-
tion. The bulk of the existing experimental work examining
recipients’ responses to gifts has focused on identifying
gifts that are better liked and appreciated (Flynn and
Adams 2009; Gino and Flynn 2011), rather than on under-
standing how gifts can change the relationship between the
gift giver and recipient (Aknin and Human 2015).
Although our findings indicate that the extent to which the
recipient likes the gift is positively related to change in re-
lationship strength, liking does not appear to drive our ef-
fect, as we did not find consistent differences in how much
recipients liked experiential and material gifts. Instead, the
gift’s emotionality is what seems to make experiential gifts
better than material gifts at strengthening
AQ3
relationships. By
examining emotions evoked during gift consumption, our
research also complements qualitative work examining the
emotion that arises during the gift exchange (Belk and
Coon 1993; Ruth et al. 1999, 2004). By studying the emo-
tion evoked from gift consumption and testing its impact
on relationships, our research thus provides new insights
into gift giving.
Future Research and Marketing Implications
Although experiences tend to be more emotional, our re-
search shows that emotionally evocative material gifts can
also strengthen relationships. Future work should explore
how possessions become associated with emotion, and
what types of possessions are most meaningful. For ex-
ample, recent research has found that items associated with
loved ones and special events carry sentimental value
(Yang and Galak 2015). Therefore, would material gifts
that commemorate experiences be more prone to evoke
emotion so as to effectively strengthen relationships? Gift
giving is a ripe context for such investigations into mean-
ingful possessions, in light of the underlying role of emo-
tion and the focus on interpersonal relationships.
Future research should also delve further into the path-
ways through which the emotions evoked from gifts can af-
fect relationships. Our findings showed that emotional gifts
that were not jointly consumed by the gift giver and recipi-
ent were equally effective at strengthening relationships. In
these instances, is the recipient linking these consumption
emotions to the relationship (e.g., vicariously sharing the
gift consumption with the giver and thereby feeling closer),
or is the process more indirect (e.g., feeling closer to a
giver who has emotionally enriched the recipient’s life)? A
related question is whether there are particular emotions
that are more connecting than others. For instance, are gifts
given out of gratitude versus guilt differentially connecting
(Chan, Mogilner, and Van Boven 2016)? And what are the
circumstances in which negative emotions are connecting?
For example, to deepen our understanding of how experi-
ential gifts can affect relationships, future research should
contrast the effects of negative emotions that are inten-
tional (e.g., fear from watching a scary movie) versus unin-
tentional (e.g., frustration due to bad service at a
restaurant), or the effects of negative emotions directed at
the experience (e.g., sadness over a tragic play) versus at
the relationship partner (e.g., anger because the partner
arrived late for the show). Prior work has shown that the
benefits of purchasing experiences over material goods for
oneself are attenuated and sometimes reversed when the
purchase outcome is negative (Nicolao et al. 2009); there-
fore, it is quite possible that the effects of unintended nega-
tive consumption emotions due to failed experiential gifts
could be particularly detrimental for relationships.
When examining the effect of gifts on relationships, one
must also consider how to measure relationship change.
Both study 1 and the Father’s Day pilot study measured re-
lationship strength at two time periods—before and after
receiving the gift—to directly assess the gift’s impact on
the relationship. In subsequent studies, participants retro-
spectively evaluated how a gift had affected their relation-
ship with the giver. Though the latter approach allowed us
to more feasibly manipulate gift type for actual gifts
received in real relationships for real gift occasions, it also
increased the possibility of participants reporting their lay
beliefs about gifts, rather than their true reactions. This
concern is alleviated, however, because the predicted pat-
tern of results was consistent across studies that used the
two-stage approach and the retrospective approach. In add-
ition, the retrospective studies showed effects that lay the-
ory would be unlikely to predict: experiential gifts improve
relationships even when not consumed with the gift giver.
Furthermore, the use of between-subject designs lessened
the potential for demand effects. Still, future research on
gift giving should strive to employ longitudinal designs
that measure relationship strength both before and after gift
exchanges to establish the role gifts play in ongoing
relationships.
Longitudinal designs could also be used to conduct a
longer-term examination of the effects of gifts on relation-
ships to further contribute to the gift-giving literature.
Across our studies, we focused on the short-term effects of
receiving a single gift. However, a gift could have a
longer-lasting effect on a relationship (Algoe et al. 2008),
and might influence future gift-giving interactions. Further,
although we did not observe a significant effect of sharing
14 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
in the consumption of the gift, it may be that the benefits
of sharing in experiential gift consumption could emerge
later on as those cherished shared memories gain greater
interpersonal value. More generally, this research direction
would substantiate the relationship-strengthening effects of
gifts over time and more broadly inform the impact of gift
giving on relationships. Across our studies, we examined
the effects of different types of gifts on relationships—con-
tingent upon a gift being given. However, the act of gift
giving in itself has social value (Ward and Chan 2015), and
it would be worthwhile to examine its role in maintaining
relationships over time, compared to relationships in which
no gifts are given.
The current research emphasizes the interpersonal bene-
fits of experiential gifts. Future work could explore poten-
tial intrapersonal benefits of giving experiences. Engaging
in relationship maintenance behaviors has been found to
increase individual well-being when these efforts are suc-
cessful in improving relationship quality, but to decrease
well-being when these efforts are unsuccessful (Baker
et al. 2012), and research has documented how personal
happiness can be gained from prosocial spending (Dunn
et al. 2008a; Hershfield et al. 2016). Because giving ex-
periential gifts is more effective at strengthening
AQ4
relation-
ships, gift givers might derive greater hedonic benefits
from giving an experience than a material good.
Furthermore, gift givers might reap personal benefits from
sharing in the experience with the recipient since giving
one’s own time can lead to greater feelings of interpersonal
connection and self-efficacy (Mogilner, Chance, and
Norton 2012).
Future research could also examine whether the rela-
tional benefits observed in this research extend to
consumer-brand relationships. For example, rather than
promoting merchandise rewards, the Starwood Hotels &
Resorts Starwood Preferred Guest loyalty program encour-
ages its members to redeem their Starpoints for “incredible
experiences” and “unforgettable events.” We see that re-
tailers, such as Sephora, Nordstrom, and Saks Fifth
Avenue, give private parties and events for their loyal
customers in addition to more material gifts, such as free
cosmetic items. Follow-up work should test whether ex-
periential rewards are more effective at strengthening
consumer-brand connections than material rewards.
Finally, companies that sell experiences, such as those in
the travel or entertainment industry, should create opportu-
nities for consumers to easily purchase experiences to give
as gifts. For example, Travelers Joy is a service that en-
ables engaged couples to build an experiential gift registry
for their honeymoon, so that family and friends can give
part of their honeymoon (e.g., a surf lesson, dinner, adven-
ture tour) as a wedding gift. Given that gift recipients pre-
fer receiving gifts from their registry over individually
selected gifts (Gino and Flynn 2011), our research implies
that such experiential gift registries will benefit gift givers,
recipients, and the companies that provide experiences.
Conclusion
Consumers frequently struggle with the challenge of
choosing what to give. Most gift-giving occasions are
therefore accompanied by a flurry of advice columns and
top 10 lists of gift ideas, as media and marketers try to help
consumers make choices that will improve their relation-
ships. This research offers simple guidance: to make your
friend, spouse, or family member feel closer to you, give
an experience.
DATA COLLECTION INFORMATION
The first author supervised the collection of data for
studies 1 and 2 by research assistants at the University of
Pennsylvania’s Wharton Behavioral Lab in summer 2013
and winter 2011, respectively. The first author managed
the data collection for studies 3 and 4 on Amazon’s
Mechanical Turk in summer 2013 and fall 2015, respect-
ively. The data were analyzed by the first author in collab-
oration with the second author.
CHAN AND MOGILNER 15
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
APPENDIX A
STUDIES 1 AND 3: INCLUSION OF OTHER
SCALE ADAPTED FROM Aron et al. (1992)
APPENDIX B
AQ5
STUDY 2: COFFEE MUGS GIVEN AS
GIFTS
APPENDIX C
STUDY 3: 30 DISCRETE EMOTIONS
MEASURED
PANAS-X General Dimension Scales
Positive affect: active, alert, attentive, determined, enthusi-
astic, excited, inspired, interested, proud, strong
Negative affect: afraid, scared, nervous, jittery, irritable,
hostile, guilty, ashamed, upset, distressed
Other positive: happy, delighted/cheerful, calm, sur-
prised, grateful
Other negative: sad, lonely, angry, disgusted, embarrassed
REFERENCES
Adams, Gabrielle S., Francis J. Flynn, and Michael I. Norton
(2012), “The Gifts We Keep on Giving: Documenting and
Destigmatizing the Regifting Taboo,” Psychological Science,
23 (10), 1145–50.
Aknin, Lara B. and Lauren J. Human (2015), “Give a Piece of
You: Gifts that Reflect Givers Promote Closeness,” Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 60, 8–16.
Relationship
before receiving
gift
Relationship
after receiving
gift
hh
hh
hh
hh
hh
hh
hh
hh
hh
16 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Algoe, Sara B., Jonathan Haidt, and Shelly Gable (2008), “Beyond
Reciprocity: Gratitude and Relationships in Everyday Life,”
Emotion, 8 (3), 425–29.
Ariely, Dan (2011), “Is It Irrational to Give Holiday Gifts?” Wall
Street Journal, December 17.
Aron, Arthur, Elaine N. Aron, and Danny Smollan (1992),
“Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale and the Structure of
Interpersonal Closeness,” Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 63 (4), 596–612.
Aron, Arthur, Elaine N. Aron, Michael Tudor, and Greg Nelson
(1991), “Close Relationships as Including Other in the Self,”
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60 (2),
241–53.
Aron, Arthur, Christina C. Norman, Elaine N. Aron, Colin
McKenna, and Richard E. Heyman (2000), “Couples’ Shared
Participation in Novel and Arousing Activites and
Experienced Relationship Quality,” Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 78 (2), 273–84.
Baker, Levi R., James K. McNulty, Nickola C. Overall, Nathaniel
M. Lambert, and Frank D. Fincham (2012), “How Do
Relationship Maintenance Behaviors Affect Individual Well-
Being? A Contextual Perspective,” Social Psychological and
Personality Science, 4 (3), 282–89.
Baumeister, Roy F. and Mark R. Leary (1995), “The Need to
Belong: Desire for Interpersonal Attachments as a
Fundamental Human Motivation,” Psychological Bulletin,
117 (3), 497–529.
Bazzini, Doris G., Elizabeth R. Stack, Penny D. Martincin, and
Carmen P. Davis (2007), “The Effect of Reminiscing about
Laughter on Relationship Satisfaction,” Motivation and
Emotion, 31 (1), 25–34.
Belk, Russell W. and Gregory S. Coon (1993), “Gift Giving as
Agapic Love: An Alternative to the Exchange Paradigm
Based on Dating Experiences,” Journal of Consumer
Research, 20 (3), 393–417.
Bhattacharjee, Amit and Cassie Mogilner (2014), “Happiness
from Ordinary and Extraordinary Experiences,” Journal of
Consumer Research, 41 (1), 1–17.
Brown, Stephanie L., Barbara L. Fredrickson, Michelle M. Wirth,
Michael J. Poulin, Elizabeth A. Meier, Emily D. Heaphy,
Michael D. Cohen, and Oliver C. Schultheiss (2009), “Social
Closeness Increases Salivary Progesterone in Humans,”
Hormones and Behavior, 56, 108–11.
Caprariello, Peter A. and Harry T. Reis (2013), “To Do, to Have,
or to Share? Valuing Experiences over Material Possessions
Depends on the Involvement of Others,” Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 104 (2), 199–215.
Carter, Travis J. and Thomas Gilovich (2010), “The Relative
Relativity of Material and Experiential Purchases,” Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 98 (1), 146–59.
— (2012), “I Am What I Do, Not What I Have: The Centrality
of Experiential Purchases to the Self-Concept,” Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 102 (6), 1304–17.
Cavanaugh, Lisa A., Francesca Gino, and Gavan J. Fitzsimons
(2015), “When Doing Good Is Bad in Gift Giving: Mis-
Predicting Appreciation of Socially Responsible Gifts,”
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
131, 178–89.
Chan, Cindy, Mogilner Cassie, and Leaf Van Boven (2016),
“Guilt, Gratitude, and Gift Giving,” working paper,
University of Toronto, Toronto ON M1C 1A4.
Chan, Cindy, Leaf Van Boven, Eduardo B. Andrade, and Dan
Ariely (2014), “Moral Violations Reduce Oral
Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24 (3),
381–86.
Clark, Margaret S. and Eli J. Finkel (2004), “Does Expressing
Emotion Promote Well-Being? It Depends on Relationship
Context,” in The Social Life of Emotions, ed. Larissa Z.
Tiedens and Colin W. Leach, New York: Cambridge
University Press, 105–26.
Clark, Margaret S. and Edward P. Lemay (2010), “Close
Relationships,” in Handbook of Social Psychology, 5th ed.,
vol. 2, ed. Susan T. Fiske, Daniel T. Gilbert, and Gardner
Lindzey, Hoboken, NJ: Wiley, 898–940.
Dai, Hengchen, Cindy Chan, and Cassie Mogilner (2016), “People
Rely Less on Consumer Reviews for Experiential Purchases
than for Material Purchases,” working paper, Washington
University, St.
Louis, MO 63130.
Derbaix, Christian and Michel T. Pham (1991), “Affective
Reactions to Consumption Situations: A Pilot Investigation,”
Journal of Economic Psychology, 12, 325–55.
Dibble, Jayson L., Timothy R. Levine, and Hee Sun Park (2012),
“The Unidimensional Relationship Closeness Scale (URCS):
Reliability and Validity Evidence for a New Measure of
Relationship Closeness,” Psychological Assessment, 24 (3),
565–72.
Dunn, Elizabeth W., Lara B. Aknin, and Michael I. Norton
(2008a), “Spending Money on Others Promotes Happiness,”
Science, 319, 1687–88.
Dunn, Elizabeth W., Jeff Huntsinger, Janetta Lun, and Stacey
Sinclair (2008b), “The Gift of Similarity: How Good and Bad
Gifts Influence Relationships,” Social Cognition, 26 (4),
469–81.
Fischer, Agneta H. and Antony S. R. Manstead (2008), “Social
Functions of Emotions,” in Handbook of Emotions, ed.
Michael Lewis, Jeannette M. Haviland-Jones, and Lisa
Feldman Barrett, New York: Guilford Press, 114–37.
Flynn, Francis J. and Gabrielle S. Adams (2009), “Money Can’t
Buy Love: Asymmetric Beliefs About Gift Price and
Feelings of Appreciation,” Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 45 (2), 404–9.
Gilovich, Thomas, Amit Kumar, and Lily Jampol (2015), “A
Wonderful Life: Experiential Consumption and the Pursuit of
Happiness,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25 (1),
152–65.
Gino, Francesca and Francis J. Flynn (2011), “Give Them What
They Want: The Benefits of Explicitness in Gift Exchange,”
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47 (5), 915–22.
Graham, Steven M., Julie Y. Huang, Margaret S. Clark, and Vicki
S. Helgeson (2008), “The Positives of Negative Emotions:
Willingness to Express Negative Emotions Promotes
Relationships,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
34 (3), 394–406.
Halvena, William J. and Morris B. Holbrook (1986), “The
Varieties of Consumption Experience: Comparing Two
Typologies of Emotion in Consumer Behavior,” Journal of
Consumer Research, 13 (3), 394–404.
Hayes, Andrew F. (2012), “PROCESS: A Versatile
Computational Tool for Observed Variable Mediation,
Moderation, and Conditional Process Modeling,” white
paper, Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210.
Hershfield, Hal E., Cassie Mogilner, and Uri Barnea (2016),
“People Who Choose Time over Money Are Happier,”
Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7 (7),
697–706.
CHAN AND MOGILNER 17
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Hutcherson, Cendri A., Emma M. Seppala, and James J. Gross
(2008), “Loving-Kindness Meditation Increases Social
Connectedness,” Emotion, 8 (5), 720–24.
Kahn, Jeffrey H., Rene´e M. Tobin, Audra E. Massey, and Jennifer
A. Anderson (2007), “Measuring Emotional Expression with
the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count,” American Journal
of Psychology, 120 (2), 263–86.
Kok, Bethany E., Kimberly A. Coffey, Michael A. Cohen, Lahnna
I. Catolino, Tanya Vacharkulksemsuk, Sara B. Algoe, Mary
Brantley, and Barbara L. Fredrickson (2013), “How Positive
Emotions Build Physical Health: Perceived Positive Social
Connections Account for the Upward Spiral between Positive
Emotions and Vagal Tone,” Psychological Science, 24,
1123–32.
Kok, Bethany E. and Barbara L. Fredrickson (2010), “Upward
Spirals of the Heart: Autonomic Flexibility, as Indexed by
Vagal Tone, Reciprocally and Prospectively Predicts Positive
Emotions and Social Connectedness,” Biological
Psychology, 85, 432–36.
Kubacka, Kaska E., Catrin Finkenaur, Caryl E. Rusbult, and Loes
Keijsers (2011), “Maintaining Close Relationships: Gratitude
as a Motivator and a Detector of Maintenance Behavior,”
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37 (10),
1362–75.
Kumar, Amit and Thomas Gilovich (2016), “To Do or to Have,
Now or Later? The Preferred Consumption Profiles of
Material and Experiential Purchases,” Journal of Consumer
Psychology, 26 (2), 169–78.
Kumar, Amit, Matthew A. Killingsworth, and Thomas Gilovich
(2014), “Waiting for Merlot: Anticipatory Consumption of
Experiential and Material Purchases,” Psychological
Science, 25 (10), 1924–31.
Laurenceau, Jean-Philippe, Lisa Feldman Barrett, and Paula R.
Pietromonaco (1998), “Intimacy as an Interpersonal Process:
The Importance of Self-Disclosure, Partner Disclosure, and
Perceived Partner Responsiveness in Interpersonal
Exchanges,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
74 (5), 1238–51.
Mogilner, Cassie (2010), “The Pursuit of Happiness: Time,
Money, and Social Connection,” Psychological Science,21
(September), 1348–54.
Mogilner, Cassie and Jennifer Aaker (2009), “‘The Time vs.
Money Effect’: Shifting Product Attitudes and Decisions
through Personal Connection,” Journal of Consumer
Research, 36 (6), 277–291.
Mogilner, Cassie, Jennifer Aaker, and Sepandar D. Kamvar
(2012), “How Happiness Affects Choice,” Journal of
Consumer Research, 39 (August), 429–43.
Mogilner, Cassie, Zoe Chance, and Michael I. Norton (2012),
“Giving Time Gives You Time,” Psychological Science,23
(10), 1233–38.
Mogilner, Cassie, Sepandar D. Kamvar, and Jennifer Aaker
(2011), “The Shifting Meaning of Happiness,” Social
Psychological and Personality Science, 2 (4), 395–402.
Nicolao, Leonardo, Julie R. Irwin, and Joseph K. Goodman
(2009), “Happiness for Sale: Do Experiential Purchases
Make Consumers Happier than Material Purchases?” Journal
of Consumer Research, 36 (2), 188–98.
Nummenmaa, Lauri, Enrico Glerean, Mikko Viinikainen, Iiro P.
J
a
askel
ainen, Riitta Hari, and Mikko Sams (2012),
“Emotions Promote Social Interaction by Synchronizing
Brain Activity across Individuals,” Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, 109 (24), 9599–604.
Pennebaker, James W., Roger J. Booth, and Martha E. Francis
(2007), Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count: LIWC [computer
software], Austin, TX: LIWC.net.
Peters, Kim and Yoshihisa Kashima (2007), “From Social Talk to
Social Action: Shaping the Social Triad with Emotion
Sharing,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,93
(5), 780–97.
Raghunathan, Rajagopal and Kim Corfman (2006), “Is Happiness
Shared Doubled and Sadness Shared Halved? Social
Influence on Enjoyment of Hedonic Experiences,” Journal of
Marketing Research, 43
(3), 386–94.
Ramanathan, Suresh and Ann L. McGill (2007), “Moment-to-
Moment and Retrospective Evaluations of an Experience,”
Journal of Consumer Research, 34 (4), 506–24.
Reis, Harry T., W. Andrew Collins, and Ellen Berscheid (2000),
“The Relationship Context of Human Behavior and
Development,” Psychological Bulletin, 126 (6), 844–72.
Richins, Marsha L. (1997), “Measuring Emotions in the
Consumption Experience,” Journal of Consumer Research,
24 (2), 127–46.
Rosenzweig, Emily and Thomas Gilovich (2012), “Buyer’s
Remorse or Missed Opportunity?” Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 102 (2), 215–23.
Rusbult, Caryl E., John M. Martz, and Christopher R. Agnew
(1998), “The Investment Model Scale: Measuring Commitment
Level, Satisfaction Level, Quality of Alternatives, and
Investment Size,” Personal Relationships, 5, 357–91.
Ruth, Julie A., Fre´de´ric F. Brunel, and Cele C. Otnes (2004), “An
Investigation of the Power of Emotions in Relationship
Realignment: The Gift Recipient’s Perspective,” Psychology
& Marketing, 21 (1), 29–52.
Ruth, Julie A., Cele C. Otnes, and Fre´de´ric F. Brunel (1999), “Gift
Receipt and the Reformulation of Interpersonal Relationships,”
Journal of Consumer Research, 25 (4), 385–402.
Sherry, John F., Jr. (1983), “Gift Giving in Anthropological
Perspective,” Journal of Consumer Research,10
(September), 157–68.
Slatcher, Richard B. and James W. Pennebaker (2006), “How Do I
Love Thee? Let Me Count the Words,” Psychological
Science, 17 (8), 660–64.
Steffel, Mary and Robyn LeBoeuf (2014), “Overindividuation in
Gift Giving: Shopping for Multiple Recipients Leads Givers
to Choose Unique but Less Preferred Gifts,” Journal of
Consumer Research, 40 (6), 1167–80.
Tooby, John and Leda Cosmides (2008), “The Evolutionary
Psychology of the Emotions and Their Relationship to
Internal Regulatory Variables,” in Handbook of Emotions,
ed. Michael Lewis, Jeannette M. Haviland-Jones, and Lisa
Feldman Barrett, New York: Guilford Press, 114–37.
Tully, Stephanie M., Hal E. Hershfield, and Tom Meyvis (2015),
“Seeking Lasting Employment with Limited Money:
Financial Constraints Increase Preference for Material Goods
over Experiences,” Journal of Consumer Research, 42 (1),
59–75.
Unity Marketing (2015), “Gifting Report 2015: The Ultimate
Guide to the Consumer Gift-Giving Market,” May 1.
US Bureau of Labor Statistics (2013), Consumer Expenditure
Survey, Washington, DC: Government Printing Office.
Van Boven, Leaf and Thomas Gilovich (2003), “To Do or to
Have? That Is the Question,” Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 85 (6), 1193–202.
Van Boven, Leaf, Joanne Kane, A. Peter McGraw, and Jeannette
Dale (2010), “Feeling Close: Emotional Intensity Reduces
18 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Perceived Psychological Distance,” Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 98 (6), 872–85.
Waldfogel, Joel (1993), “The Deadweight Loss of Christmas,”
American Economic Review, 85 (5), 1328–36.
Ward, Morgan K. and Susan M. Broniarczyk (in press), “Ask and
You Shall (Not) Receive: Close Friends Prioritize Relational
Signaling over Recipient Preferences in Their Gift Choices,”
Journal of Marketing Research.
Ward, Morgan K. and Cindy Chan (2015), “Gift Giving,” in
Cambridge Handbook of Consumer Psychology, ed. Michael
I. Norton, Derek D. Rucker, and Cait Lamberton, Cambridge
MA: Cambridge Press, 398–418.
Watson, David L. and Lee A. Clark (1994), The PANAS-X:
Manual for the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule:
Expanded Form, Ames, Iowa: University of Iowa.
Weidman, Aaron C. and Elizabeth W. Dunn (2016), The Unsung
Benefits of Material Things: Material Purchases Provide More
Frequent Momentary Happiness than Experiential Purchases,”
Social Psychology and Personality Science, 7 (4), 390–99.
Yang, Yang and Jeff Galak (2015), “Sentimental Value and Its
Influence on Hedonic Adaptation,” Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 109 (5), 767–90.
Zhang, Yan and Nicholas Epley (2012), “Exaggerated,
Mispredicted, and Misplaced: When ‘It’s the Thought That
Counts’ in Gift Exchanges,” Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 141 (4), 667–81.
Zhao, Xinshu, John G. Lynch Jr., and Qimei Chen (2010),
“Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and Truths about
Mediation Analysis,” Journal of Consumer Research,37
(April), 197–206.
CHAN AND MOGILNER 19
by guest on January 9, 2017http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from