RACE, PROSECUTORS, AND JURIES: THE DEATH PENALTY
IN
TENNESSEE
J
OHN M. SCHEB II, WILLIAM LYONS, AND KRISTIN A. WAGERS
The behavior of prosecutors and juries with respect to the decision to impose the death penal-
ty is examined. Using data on 968 first-degree murder convictions in Tennessee from 1977
to 2006, models of prosecutorial and jury behavior are constructed with a focus on the role
of the defendant’s race and the victim’s race. Once significant controls are introduced, nei-
ther the race of the defendant nor that of the victim is a significant predictor of prosecutors’
decisions to seek the death penalty. Nor is the race of the victim a significant predictor of the
jury’s decision to impose the death penalty. The defendant’s race is a significant, albeit weak,
predictor of jury behavior, but only because white defendants are more likely to be sentenced
to death. While a number of other variables are significantly related to both prosecutorial and
jury behavior, much of the variance in these decisions remains unexplained.
n Furman v. Georgia (1972), the United States Supreme Court held that Georgia’s
death penalty statute violated the Eighth Amendment’s ban on “cruel and unusu-
al punishments.” Of central concern to the Court was the perceived inadequacy of
procedures in capital cases and, in particular, the unfettered discretion vested in trial
juries regarding the imposition of the ultimate punishment. The resulting national
moratorium on the death penalty lasted until the Court approved Georgia’s revised
death penalty statute in
Gregg v. Georgia (1976). The revised statute adopted new
procedures designed to render the imposition of the death penalty less arbitrary and
capricious. Most notable of these was the “bifurcated trial,” in which the guilt/inno-
cence phase of the trial is separated from the penalty phase.
In the wake of
Furman and Gregg, a majority of states enacted new capital-pun-
ishment laws modeled after the new Georgia statute. Today, thirty-seven states and
the federal government have death penalty statutes. While these laws vary somewhat
procedurally, all are based on the bifurcated-trial model upheld in
Gregg v. Georgia.
All of these statutes are predicated on the notion that structuring the jury’s discretion
in death penalty cases reduces the potential for arbitrariness and discrimination to a
constitutionally acceptable level.
In the American system of criminal justice, pr
osecutors ar
e vested with enor
-
mous discr
etion. Subject to minimal pr
ocedural checks thr
ough grand jur
y pr
oceed
-
ings, pr
eliminar
y hearings, or both, pr
osecutors decide whether to bring criminal
char
ges and which char
ges to bring. In jurisdictions that have the death penalty
,
pr
osecutors must decide whether to seek the death penalty in cases wher
e defendants
ar
e char
ged with first-degr
ee mur
der
. Befor
e such trials begin, prosecutors must for-
mally notify trial cour
ts and defendants that they intend to seek the death penalty
upon conviction.
Gr
egg v
. Geor
gia
and subsequent judicial decisions have done little
THE JUSTICE SYSTEM JOURNAL, VOL. 29, NUMBER 3 (2008)
I
RACE, PROSECUTORS, AND JURIES: THE DEATH PENALTY IN TENNESSEE 339
if anything to reduce prosecutorial discretion to seek the death penalty. We seek
here to determine the case characteristics that influence prosecutors to seek the death
penalty and those that lead juries to impose it.
RACE AND THE DEATH PENALTY
One of the long-standing concerns about the administration of the death penalty has
been the potential for racial discrimination. Many have documented racial inequal-
ity in the administration of the death penalty (see Mangum, 1940; Garfinkel, 1949;
Johnson, 1957). However, Kleck (1981) criticized these early studies for failing to
utilize adequate controls. Justice William O. Douglas expressed concern for racial bias
in his concurring opinion in
Furman, where he characterized the old Georgia capital-
punishment statute as “pregnant with discrimination” (408 U.S. at 257). Supporters
of the death penalty were hopeful that the structured discretion model established by
the new post-
Furman statutes would lead to race-neutral application of the death
penalty. However, social-science research in this area has cast doubt on this aspira-
tion. While there is little evidence of racial discrimination in terms of the race of
defendants, there is substantial evidence of disparity with regard to the race of victims
(e.g., Baldus, Pulaski, and Woodworth, 1983, 1986; Lempert, 1983; Radelet and
Pierce, 1985; Smith, 1987; Ekland-Olson, 1988; Vito and Keil, 1988).
Summarizing the thrust of research in this area, Radelet and Borg (2000:47)
conclude that “the death penalty is between three and four times more likely to be
imposed in cases in which the victim is white rather than black.” There is also some
evidence of an interaction between the race of defendants and that of victims. Some
studies have found that the greatest disparity in the rate at which the death penalty
is sought or imposed exists between cases where black defendants are alleged to have
killed white victims and those where black defendants are alleged to have killed black
victims (see, e.g., Baldus, Pulaski, and Wordworth, 1983, 1986; Paternoster, 1983,
1984). To the extent that prosecutors and juries reflect dominant attitudes of their
communities, such disparities would seem quite plausible. On the other hand, some
researchers argue that apparent racial differences in the administration of the death
penalty can be explained in terms of nonracial case characteristics (see, e.g.,
Heilbrun, Foster, and Golden, 1989; Klein and Rolph, 1991).
Pr
osecutorial Behavior
. Pr
osecutors possess the discretion to “formulate charges that
deter
mine whether or not the death penalty is permitted if a conviction is obtained”
and to pr
opose a sentence of life in exchange for a guilty plea in cases wher
e the
defendant is eligible for the death penalty (Zeisel, 1981:466). Usually elected of
fi
-
cials, pr
osecutors must be attentive to their constituency’
s r
eaction to crime, which
may influence the pr
osecutor
s decision to seek the death penalty
, accept a guilty plea
for a lesser sentence, or agr
ee to a plea bar
gain in exchange for testimony against
another defendant (Bowers, 1984). Bowers (1984:339, 345) suggests that elected
pr
osecutors ar
e mor
e likely to seek the death penalty, given the legal requirements to
340 THE JUSTICE SYSTEM JOURNAL
do so, when there is public outcry for capital punishment in a given case or when
there is pressure from the police department. Therefore, he asserts that the unfettered
discretion exercised by prosecutors may be a vehicle of “arbitrariness and discrimina-
tion” in the early stages of capital cases (Bowers 1984:340). In a study of death penal-
ty cases in North Carolina, Nakell and Hardy (1987) found that a prosecutor’s deci-
sion to seek the death penalty largely depended on which prosecutor was assigned to
any given case, thus buttressing previously expressed concerns about arbitrariness and
the potential for discrimination.
Focusing specifically on race as a determinant of prosecutorial behavior,
Paternoster analyzed 300 homicide cases and found that prosecutors were four-and-a-
half times more likely to seek the death penalty when black defendants have white
victims. This would seem to suggest, as does other research, that “black
offender/white victim homicides are treated as more aggravated killings, and black
offender/black victim homicides are treated as less aggravated deaths” (Paternoster,
1984: 453). In another analysis, Paternoster found that prosecutors were forty times
more likely to seek the death penalty when black defendants were accused of killing
white victims than when black defendants were accused of killing other African-
Americans (Paternoster, 1983: 766). Similarly, after examining data from five South
Carolina counties, Johnson (2003) found the race of the victim to be a significant
predictor of prosecutors’ decision to file a notice to seek the death penalty.
Jury Behavior. Both prosecutors and jurors rely on their own socially conditioned
views in the criminal justice system. Bowers (1984:338-39) found jurors “embody
community sentiments” by having their own preconceived notions about members of
society, and as a result, “jurors have difficulty replacing their socially conditioned views
of victims and offenders with strictly legal considerations, especially for the crimes they
find most shocking and abhorrent”; this allows “extralegal” factors to intrude on the
interpretation of aggravating and mitigating factors presented at sentencing.
Gross and Mauro (1989:112) found that for jurors to sentence a defendant to
death, they must be “particularly horrified by the crime and perhaps frightened by the
defendant. As the jurors review the circumstances of the crime, they will inevitably
consider the personal characteristics of the defendant, and the defendant’s race might
carry some weight.” They argue that the one aspect of a trial that influences a juror’s
decision to sentence a defendant to death is the ability to identify with the victim,
rather than seeing the victim as a stranger
. In mo
der
n society
, where social classes
tend to be segr
egated and jurors tend to be Caucasian, “jurors are more likely to be
hor
rified by the killing of a white than of a black, and more likely to act against the
killer of a white than the killer of a black,” which is “a natural pr
oduct of the patterns
of inter
racial relations in society” (Gross and Mauro, 1989:113).
In ter
ms of the race of the defendant, racial stereotypes become relevant in the
sentencing phase of capital jur
y trials. In many states, juries are “allowed or required
to deter
mine whether the defendant will be dangerous in the future, even in states
where future dangerousness is not formally part of the sentencing process” (Johnson
RACE, PROSECUTORS, AND JURIES: THE DEATH PENALTY IN TENNESSEE 341
2003:136), and, according to a 1990 National Opinion Research Center survey, a
majority of whites feel African-Americans are more “prone to violence than whites”
(Johnson 2003:136). Given these survey results, Johnson (2003:137) suggests that
many Caucasian jurors think blacks are more likely to be involved in violent behav-
ior, and as a result, these jurors are more likely to impose a death sentence. Johnson
also believes that mitigating factors, specifically good character, are less likely to be
given significant weight in the sentencing phase for blacks because jurors “attribute
fewer positive traits to people of color, see them as less intelligent, less hard-working
and less good” than whites. Therefore, there is support for the conclusion that “jurors
are more likely to perceive the presence of aggravating factors in black defendant
cases than they do in white defendant cases with equal evidence of aggravation, and
they are less likely to give weight to mitigating factors” (Johnson, 2003:137).
THE TENNESSEE STUDY
Clearly, the two loci of potential bias in the imposition of death sentences are 1) the
prosecutor’s decision to seek the death penalty, which triggers the special death penal-
ty procedures, and 2) the jury’s decision to sentence a convicted murderer to death.
Prior research has tended to focus on one or the other, but it is important to examine
both together. Our study focuses on Tennessee, where in fall 2006 the Administrative
Office of the Courts released data on 986 first-degree murder convictions dating from
1977, when T
ennessee reformed its death penalty law, to early 2006. The data were
taken from reports submitted by trial judges across the state in compliance with
Tennessee Supreme Court Rule 12, which requires judges to complete detailed reports
on cases in which defendants are convicted of first-degree murder.
While the Tennessee data set is far from perfect, it is a useful tool with which
to analyze prosecutorial and jury behavior. The data set is quite comprehensive in
terms of the variables included, allowing us to utilize a variety of controls.
Specifically, we have data on demographic characteristics of perpetrators and victims.
We also have information about the nature and location of the homicide, the means
by which death was inflicted, and the perpetrator’s motive, relationship to the victim,
and criminal history. The data set also contains the nature of the evidence before the
jury, including whether the defendant confessed to the homicide. Finally, it also
includes records of the statutory aggravating and mitigating factors found by juries.
The Rule 12 database has been criticized as being incomplete because trial
judges were not always careful or diligent in filling out questionnaires. Indeed, it has
been suggested that as many as 20 percent of first-degree murder cases may be miss-
ing from the data set (see
State of Tennessee v. Godsey [2001], Birch. J., dissenting).
At the present time, we have no way of assessing how extensive are the missing data;
for present purposes, we assume any missing data to be randomly distributed across
variables and cases.
Death Sentences and Race in Tennessee: A First Look. Ninety-eight percent of
defendants in these cases were either black (45.5 percent) or white (52.5 percent).
342 THE JUSTICE SYSTEM JOURNAL
The 2 percent who were Hispanic, Asian, Native American, or “other” have been
excluded from the analysis. Similarly, 97.5 percent of victims were either black (33.6
percent) or white (63.9 percent); the remaining 2.5 percent have been excluded.
Looking first at the race of defendants, we note that 19.2 percent of white defendants
are sentenced to death as compared with only 11.5 percent of black defendants (see
Table 1). Clearly, there is a racial discrepancy, but in the direction opposite from
what many might expect. However, with respect to the race of victims, the opposite
is true: 18.5 percent of cases involving white victims led to the death penalty, where-
as only 9.8 percent of cases involving black victims resulted in death sentences.
Prosecutors were actually more likely to seek the death penalty in cases involving
white defendants, but they were also more likely to seek the death penalty when vic-
tims were white. Juries were more likely to impose the death penalty both in cases
involving white defendants and in cases involving white victims.
If we intersect the race of the victim with the race of the defendant (see Table
2), we find that black defendants accused of killing black victims receive the death
penalty about 10 percent of the time, while black defendants accused of killing white
persons are sentenced to death 15 percent of the time. Similarly, white defendants
accused of murdering blacks get the death penalty about 12 percent of the time, while
whites accused of killing other whites are sentenced to death 19 percent of the time.
However, when we separate the prosecutorial function from the jury function, we see
that the disparity in terms of the victim’s race exists in the decisions of prosecutors to
seek the death penalty, not in juries’ decisions to impose it. The data show that blacks
accused of killing black victims were just about as likely—actually, slightly more like-
ly—to be sentenced to death by juries as blacks who killed whites. Similarly, whites
who killed blacks were slightly more likely to be sentenced to death by juries than
whites who killed whites. On the other hand, when blacks are accused of killing
blacks, prosecutors seek the death penalty only about 25 percent of the time; when
blacks are accused of killing whites, the rate jumps to 39 percent. Likewise, when
whites are accused of killing blacks, prosecutors seek death only about 24 percent of
Table 1
Prosecutorial and Jury Behavior by Race of Defendant and Victim
Defendant Victim All
Black White Black White
Sentenced to death 11.5% 19.2% 9.8% 18.5% 15.8%
(% of all cases)
Prosecutor seeks death penalty 30.3 39.4 24.7 39.9 35.3
(% of all cases)
Death sentence imposed by jury 37.3 48.1 39.7 46.0 44.2
(% of cases where death penalty is sought)
RACE, PROSECUTORS, AND JURIES: THE DEATH PENALTY IN TENNESSEE 343
the time; when whites are charged with killing other whites, the rate increases to
nearly 40 percent. Thus, it would appear that, if there is racial discrimination with
respect to victims, it occurs more in the prosecutorial office than at the jury stage.
This significant difference between prosecutors and juries would make sense given the
enormous discretion vested in prosecutors, as contrasted with the much more struc-
tured decision making of juries under current death penalty law.
Introducing Controls. Before concluding that Tennessee prosecutors engage in racial
discrimination with respect to the administration of capital punishment, we need to
look at other factors that might explain the apparent disparities noted above.
Therefore, we constructed more complex statistical models incorporating the data on
defendant characteristics, victim characteristics, crime characteristics, and case char-
acteristics as controls with the goal of generating the most parsimonious model, that
is, one that maximizes explanatory power with a minimal number of additional vari-
ables.
1
We wished to see whether the racial variables remain significant when incor-
porated into such a model. We find that, once appropriate controls are introduced,
including the place, method, motive of the homicide, the nature of the evidence,
whether there are multiple victims, and the defendant’s criminal history,
the race of
the defendant and the race of the victim are not significant predictors of a death penalty out-
come
(see Table 3).
2
With the exception of the defendant’s race and the victim’s race, all variables in
the model are significant at the .05 level, which means that the probability of the
observed relationship occurring randomly is less than 5 percent (see Table 3). Among
Table 2
Prosecutorial and Jury Behavior by Race of Defendant by Race of Victim
Black Def. Black Def. White Def. White Def.
/Black /White /White /White
Victim Victim Victim Victim All
Sentenced to death 9.7% 15.0% 11.8% 19.0% 15.8%
(% of all cases)
Prosecutor seeks death penalty 24.7 39.0 23.5 39.7 35.3
(% of all cases)
Death sentence imposed by jury 39.4 38.5 50.0 47.2 44.2
(% of cases where d.p. is sought)
1
Details about the statistical tests performed, and statistical results, are available from the authors on request.
2
Specifically, the other independent variables are 1) whether the defendant confessed to murder; 2) whether
the defendant had thr
ee or mor
e prior felonies; 3) whether the defendant made incriminating statements; 4)
whether the metho
d of killing was dr
owning; 5) whether the method of killing was throat slashing; 6) whether
the murder was committed for pecuniary or other gain; 7) whether the murder was committed for sexual or other
gratification; 8) whether the mur
der was committed to escape appr
ehension or punishment; 9) whether ther
e
were three or more victims of the murder; 10) whether the victim was killed at place of business or employment;
and 11) whether the victim was killed in a field, woods, or rural area.
344 THE JUSTICE SYSTEM JOURNAL
the significant predictors, all of the effects are in the expected direction; that is, all these
factors increase the probability that a death sentence will be imposed. A nontechnical
way of gauging the overall explanator
y power of the model is to look at the improve-
ment in the ability to “pr
edict” correctly the number of cases in which the defendant
r
eceives the death penalty. In the absence of the model, we are able to correctly predict
individual cases 84 per
cent of the time simply on the basis that we know that 84 per-
cent of defendants did r
eceive the death penalty; that is, if we pr
edict that no defen-
dants receive the death penalty, we are correct 84 percent of the time. The model
improves our ability to predict correctly to 87 percent, a marginal improvement at best.
But what of the prosecutor’s decision to seek the death penalty? Here we should
note that the rate at which prosecutors seek the death penalty varies by county from
6 percent to 100 percent. Of course, at the extremes are counties with very small
Table 3
Multivariate Model: Whether Death Penalty is Imposed (n=968)
A priori Predicted
Observed Not Sentenced to Death Sentenced to Death Percent Correct
Not sentenced to death 815 0 100.0
Sentenced to death 153 0 .0
Overall 968 0 84.2
Model Predicted
Observed Not Sentenced to Death Sentenced to Death Percent Correct
Not sentenced to death 792 23 97.2
Sentenced to death 100 53 34.6
Overall 892 77 87.3
Table 4
Multivariate Model: Prosecutors’ Decisions to Seek the Death Penalty (n=968)
A priori Predicted
Observed Does not seek DP Seeks DP Percent Correct
Does not seek DP 626 0 100.0
Seeks DP 342 0 .0
Overall 968 0 64.7
Model Predicted
Observed Does not seek DP Seeks DP Percent Correct
Does not seek DP 574 52 91.7
Seeks DP 213 129 37.7
Overall 787 181 72.6
RACE, PROSECUTORS, AND JURIES: THE DEATH PENALTY IN TENNESSEE 345
numbers of cases. But looking at the counties with the most cases, one still sees
tremendous variation. In Shelby County, which contains Memphis, prosecutors
sought the death penalty 52 percent of the time, while in Davidson County
(Nashville) prosecutors pursued capital punishment only 12 per
cent of the time. To
some extent, this variance may be attributed to varying prosecutorial philosophies,
but it likely also represents political factors that vary from county to county.
When we take into account the other controls used in the overall model, the
defendant’s race and the victim’s race become nonsignificant (see Table 4). This
model performs better in terms of predictiveness. In the absence of the model, we are
able to make correct predictions about 65 percent of the time, but the model
impr
oves the rate of correct predictions to roughly 73 percent.
The results are similar for a jury’s decision to impose the death penalty (see
Table 5). When we examine only the subsample of cases (35 percent) where prose-
cutors actually sought capital punishment, we find, as we did with the model of prose-
cutor behavior, that the race of the victim is an insignificant predictor of jury behav-
ior once the other variables are taken into account. The race of the defendant is sig-
nificant, although the relationship is weak, but the effect is opposite of what some
might expect in that being an African-American defendant actually reduces the
chances of being sentenced to death. The model performs reasonably well, improv-
ing our ability to correctly predict who will get the death penalty from 56 percent to
75 percent. Thus, it would seem that juries are more predictable than prosecutors.
CONCLUSION
Few topics have generated as much concern as has the death penalty. Even if one
accepts the legitimacy of capital punishment, any systematic bias in its application
erodes claims that it has a role in furthering justice. Despite conventional wisdom to
Table 5
Multivariate Model: Jury Imposition of Death Penalty (n=342)
A priori Predicted
Observed Does not impose DP Imposes DP Percent Correct
Does not impose DP 191 0 100.0
Imposes DP 151 0 .0
Overall 342 0 55.8
Model Predicted
Observed Does not impose DP Imposes DP Percent Correct
Does not impose DP 156 35 81.7
Imposes DP 49 102 67.5
Overall 205 137 75.4
346 THE JUSTICE SYSTEM JOURNAL
the contrary, our study of a large population of first-degree murder convictions in a
southern state produced no evidence that race is a significant factor in either prose-
cutors’ decisions to seek the death penalty or juries’ decisions to impose it at sentenc-
ing. This is in no way an argument for the legitimacy of the death penalty. Indeed,
our data suggest that, despite reforms, application of the death penalty in Tennessee
remains quite unpredictable, and this in itself may well be cause for concern.
jsj
REFERENCES
Baldus, D. C., C. A. Pulaski, Jr., and G. Woodworth (1986). “Arbitrariness and Discrimination
in the Administration of the Death Penalty: A Challenge to State Supreme Courts,” 15
Stetson Law Review 133.
_
___
(1983). “Comparative Review of Death Sentences: An Empirical Study of the Georgia
Experience,” 74
Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology 661.
Bowers, W. (1984).
Legal Homicide: Death as Punishment in America, 1864-1982. Boston:
Northeastern University Press.
Ekland-Olson, S. (1988). “Structured Discretion, Racial Bias, and the Death Penalty: The
First Decade After
Furman in Texas,” 69 Social Science Quarterly 853.
Garfinkel, H. (1949). “Research Note on Inter- and Intra-Racial Homicides,” 27
Social Forces
369.
Gross, S. R., and R. Mauro (1989).
Death and Discrimination: Racial Disparities in Capital
Sentencing.
Boston: Northeastern University Press.
Heilbrun, A. B., Jr., A. Foster, and J. Golden (1989). “The Death Sentence in Georgia, 1974-
1987: Criminal Justice or Racial Injustice?” 16 Criminal Justice and Behavior 139.
Johnson, E. (1957). “Selective Forces in Capital Punishment,” 36
Social Forces 165.
Johnson, S. (2003). “Race and Capital Punishment.” In S. P. Garvey (ed.),
Beyond Repair?:
America’s Death Penalty
. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Kleck, G. (1981). “Racial Discrimination in Criminal Sentencing: A Critical Evaluation of
the Evidence with Additional Evidence on the Death Penalty,” 46 American Sociological
Review
783.
Klein, S. P., and J. E. Rolph (1991). “Relationship of Offender and Victim Race to Death
Penalty Sentences in California,” 32
Jurimetrics Journal 33.
Lemper
t, R. (1983). “Capital Punishment in the ‘80s: Reflections on the Symposium,” 74
Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology 1101.
Mangum, C. (1940).
The Legal Status of the Negro. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina
Pr
ess.
Nakel, B., and K. Hardy (1987). The Arbitrariness of the Death Penalty. Philadelphia: Temple
University Press.
Pater
noster
, R. (1984). “Pr
osecutorial Discretion in Requesting the Death Penalty: A Case of
Victim-Based Discrimination,” 18
Law and Society Review 437.
RACE, PROSECUTORS, AND JURIES: THE DEATH PENALTY IN TENNESSEE 347
____
(1983). “Race of Victim and Location of Crime: The Decision to Seek the Death Penalty
in South Carolina,” 74
Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology 754.
Radelet, M., and M. Borg (2000). “The Changing Nature of Death Penalty Debates,” 26
Annual Review of Sociology 43.
Radelet, M. L., and G. L. Pierce (1985). “Race and Prosecutorial Discretion in Homicide
Cases,” 19 Law and Society Review 587.
Smith, M. (1987). “Patterns of Discrimination in Assessments of the Death Penalty: The Case
of Louisiana,” 15 Journal of Criminal Justice 279.
Vito, G. F., and T. J. Keil (1988). “Capital Sentencing in Kentucky: An Analysis of the Factors
Influencing Decision Making in the Post-Gregg Period,” 79 Journal of Criminal Law and
Criminology
483.
Zeisel, H. (1981). “Race Bias in the Administration of the Death Penalty: The Florida
Experience,” 95 Harvard Law Review 456.
CASES CITED
Gregg v. Georgia, 428 U.S. 153 (1976).
Furman v. Georgia, 408 U.S. 238 (1972).
State of Tennessee v. Godsey, 60 S.W.3d 759 (2001).