What “Counts” as Educational
Policy? Notes toward a New
Paradigm
JEAN ANYON
City University of New York
In my first article as a young PhD, which was published in the Harvard Educa-
tional Review, I argued that high school U.S. history curriculum, as repre-
sented in widely used textbooks, excises and thereby defines out of existence
radical responses American workers have had to the problems they face on the
job and in their communities (Anyon, 1979). This educational excision is one
way that schooling mitigates against the development of working-class con-
sciousness.
In empirical and theoretical work since then, I have investigated knowledge
and pedagogical experiences made available to students in different social-
class contexts (1980, 1981), and have attempted to understand the conse-
quences of ways we conceptualize urban education, urban school reform, and
neighborhood poverty. Recent arguments have aimed at unseating simplistic
notions of the causes of urban poverty and low achievement in city districts,
and explicating unexplored relations between urban education and move
-
ments for social change (e.g., 1995, 1997, 2005).
In this chapter I think about education policy over the seventy-five years of
Harvard Educational Review publication. During these decades, many K–12 pol
-
icies have been written and implemented by federal, state, and local govern
-
ments. Some of these have aimed at improving education in America’s cities
and are my primary focus. Over the years, dominant strategies called upon to
improve urban schools have included curricular, administrative, and funding
reforms, as well as increases in educational opportunity and district/school
accountability.
A historical examination of policies can inform decisions we make today.
Policy failures, for example, may demonstrate that we need to rethink strate
-
65
Harvard Educational Review Vol. 75 No. 1 Spring 2005
Copyright © by the President and Fellows of Harvard College
gies we choose in our long-term attempts to solve the problems of school and
student achievement in urban districts. Indeed, I will argue that the quality of
education in city schools is a complex problem, and education policy as histor
-
ically conceived has not been adequate to the task of increasing urban school
achievement to acceptable levels. Academic learning in city schools is un
-
doubtedly higher than in, say, 1900, yet there is still no large urban district
that can demonstrate high achievement in even half its students or schools.
Noting this failure of educational policy to render most urban schools high-
quality institutions, I ask, what should count as educational policy? As in any at
-
tempt to resolve complex issues, workable solutions can only be generated by
an understanding of underlying causes.
The diagnosis I provide is based on analyses completed for my book, Radical
Possibilities: Public Policy, Urban Education, and a New Social Movement (Anyon,
2005). In this book I examine federal and regional mandates that affect eco
-
nomic and social opportunities available to the urban poor. I find that despite
stated intentions, federal and metropolitan policies and arrangements gener-
ally restrict opportunities available to city residents and neighborhoods. I
show how job, wage, housing, tax, and transportation policies maintain minor-
ity poverty in urban neighborhoods, and thereby create environments that
overwhelm the potential of educational policy to create systemic, sustained
improvements in the schools. For example, policies such as minimum wage
statutes that yield full-time pay below the poverty level, and affordable hous-
ing and transportation policies that segregate low-income workers of color in
urban areas but industrial and other job development in far-flung suburbs
where public transit routes do not reach, are all culpable.
In order to solve the systemic problems of urban education, then, I argue in
the book and will argue here that we need not only better schools but
also the reform of these public policies. Rules and regulations regarding
teaching, curriculum, and assessment certainly are important, but policies to
eliminate poverty-wage work and housing segregation (for example) should
be part of the educational policy panoply as well, for these have consequences
for urban education at least as profound as curriculum, pedagogy, and testing.
In the sections that follow I describe major K–12 education policies that
have been implemented over the years to attempt to improve urban educa
-
tion, and then discuss several federal and metro-area policies and practices
that limit the potential and success of these strategies. I also report hopeful
new research suggesting that even modest income and other family supports
typically improve low-income students’ academic achievement. I end by argu
-
ing that, given this power of economic access to influence educational out
-
comes, strategies to support economic opportunity and development for ur
-
ban residents and neighborhoods should be among the policies we consider
in our attempts to improve urban schools and districts. Just as in affluent sub
-
urban districts where economic strength is the engine of educational reform,
so it would be in urban districts where resident and neighborhood affluence
Harvard Educational Review
66
would support and retool the schools. I begin with an overview of education
policy as typically conceived.
Education Policies
Over the last seventy-five years or so, federal policies have attempted various
strategies to improve city education. The first federal policy aimed at working-
class populations was the Smith-Hughes Act of 1917, which provided funds to
prepare students in industrialized areas for working-class jobs through voca
-
tional programs. Variants of this policy continued throughout the twentieth
century, in the Vocational Education Acts of 1963, 1984, and 1998, and in the
School-to-Work Opportunity Act of 1994 and the later federal legislation in
which it was subsumed.
Some federal education policies have attempted to improve urban educa
-
tion by making funding available for increased curriculum materials and li
-
braries, early childhood classes, and various types of programmatic innova-
tions in city schools. Head Start in 1965, Follow Through in 1967, and, to a
lesser extent, Title IX, which banned sex discrimination in 1972, brought and
instigated new curricula and programs into city districts.
1
These policies were
intended to increase student access and/or achievement by upgrading
curricular resources and experiences.
Other federal K–12 policies have aimed specifically at increasing educa-
tional equity. The 1954 Brown decision (which committed the federal govern-
ment to desegregation as a policy stance), the Elementary and Secondary Ed-
ucation Act of 1965 (ESEA), the Bilingual Act in 1968, Title IX in 1972, and
the Education for All Handicapped Children Act in 1975 opened doors to ac-
ademic experiences for previously underserved K–12 students. These poli
-
cies are generally thought to have expanded urban students’ educational op
-
portunities.
More recent federal education policies to improve schooling with urban
students and teachers often a target have called for increased academic
standards and requirements, standardized testing, and professional develop
-
ment of teachers. These policies were recommended by the influential report
A Nation at Risk, commissioned by President Ronald Reagan and published in
1983. The emphasis on increased academic standards was part of an effort to
support business needs for well-prepared workers and employees. The re
-
port’s recommendations for higher standards and increased testing were in
-
troduced as policy in 1994 and 1996 as part of the Goals 2000 legislation. In
2001 these goals were instantiated as federal mandates in the No Child Left
Behind Act (NCLB). Privatization of education via nonpublic providers when
K–12 schools fail is a subtextual education policy in NCLB (Conley, 2003;
Cross, 2004; Stein, 2004).
It is important to note that federal education policies intended to improve
urban schools did not take aim at the economic arrangements and practices
What “Counts” as Educational Policy?
jean anyon
67
that themselves produced the poverty in which city schools were embedded.
Despite increases in educational opportunity, the effects of almost a century
of educational policies on urban school and student achievement have, by
most accounts, been disappointing.
The first state policies regarding the education of America’s urban (and ru
-
ral) poor emerged earlier than federal ones. What has counted as state educa
-
tion policy regarding poor students can be said to have begun with mid- to
late-nineteenth century insertions into state constitutions of the right of all
students to a free, “thorough,” “efficient,” or “useful” education (Odden &
Picus, 1992). Following these insertions and until the 1970s, however, most
state education policies did not focus specifically on urban education. State
mandates typically set regulations and requirements for school systems,
teacher and administrator preparation, and school funding (through prop
-
erty taxes). During the 1970s and 1980s, lawsuits challenging state education
funding systems brought increased attention to city schools and districts. State
urban education policy in these decades involved various kinds of efforts, in-
cluding school-based management and basic skills mandates. In the 1990s,
state policies attempted to align education standards and regulations with fed-
eral ones, mandated curriculum and teacher licensure reform, and closely
monitored urban districts. As legal challenges to state systems have led to in-
creased funding of city schools, states have imposed stricter academic and
graduation requirements, as well as multigrade and multisubject standardized
testing. Quasi-privatization policies supporting charter schools, vouchers, and
other school choice programs have also been a state strategy to attempt to
improve the education of urban children by offering them a choice of schools
to attend (Conley, 2003).
Over the decades, federal and state policies codified an increasing number
of requirements that urban schools and districts must meet. Local govern
-
ments and educational bureaucracies have undertaken a plethora of pro
-
grams to attempt to meet those guidelines. Local districts have also mounted
school reform projects in response to local social conditions and political
pressure from parents and communities. Most local initiatives have been
curricular, pedagogical, and administrative.
During the Progressive Era, cities consolidated and professionalized their
school systems and personnel, introduced programs like the Gary Plan to pre
-
pare students for the industrial experience, increased access to high school,
organized educational opportunities for immigrant parents, and sometimes
fed, bathed, and clothed poor children. During the decade of the Great De
-
pression, most large cities retrenched and severely cut educational social ser
-
vice and academic programs, as local tax receipts plummeted and banks that
offered loans demanded broad cuts in education. During the 1960s, many ur
-
ban districts were weakened further as most remaining businesses and jobs
moved to the suburbs, decimating the urban property tax base (Anyon, 1997;
Ravitch, 2000; Tyack, 1974; Wrigley, 1982).
Harvard Educational Review
68
Since the 1970s, in response to federal, judicial, and state mandates, urban
districts have bused students to meet racial integration guidelines, decentral
-
ized authority to increase community participation, and created magnet
schools to attempt to attract middle-class parents. Other local policies that
have been attempted to improve achievement are a multitude of reform pro
-
grams or “school improvement projects,” student retention services, privatiza
-
tion of educational offerings, vouchers and magnets, mayoral control, small
schools, and curriculum standardization and evaluation through testing. The
social context of these policies has included pressure to be accountable in the
wake of increased funding, as well as community and corporate demands for
better schools. None of the local policies has focused on the poverty of
families or neighborhoods.
One way to evaluate this long run of education policy is to compare the
achievement of urban students at the beginning of the twentieth and twenty-
first centuries. Although achievement is higher now in that larger percentages
of students remain in school past the elementary years than in 1900, I would
argue that the improvement is relative and illusory. That is, while in the early
twentieth century relatively few urban poor students went beyond fifth grade,
the vast majority did not require further education to find employment in in-
dustries that could lead to middle-class income (Anyon, 1997; Ayres, 1909).
Currently, relatively few urban poor students go past ninth grade: The gradua-
tion rates in large comprehensive inner-city high schools are abysmally low. In
fourteen such New York City schools, for example, only 10 percent to 20 per-
cent of ninth graders in 1996 graduated four years later (Fine, 2001; Greene
2001; Miao & Haney, 2004).
2
Despite the fact that low-income individuals des-
perately need a college degree to find decent employment, only 7 percent
obtain a bachelors degree by age twenty-six (Education Trust, 2001; Mishel,
Bernstein, & Schmitt, 2001). So, in relation to the needs of low-income stu
-
dents, urban districts fail their students with more egregious consequences
now than in the early twentieth century.
Given the plethora of federal, state, and local education policies aimed at
urban schools and the current widely acknowledged necessity of high-quality
education for all, why have most urban schools and districts not been able to
provide such an education for their students?
Barriers to High-Quality Public Education in Cities
There are multiple causes of low-quality schooling in urban areas, and educa
-
tion policies as heretofore conceived address only a few. Education policy has
not addressed the neighborhood poverty that surrounds and invades urban
schools with low expectations and cynicism. Education policy has not ad
-
dressed the unemployment and joblessness of families who will have few if any
resources for the further education of their children, even if they excel in
K–12 classes.
What “Counts” as Educational Policy?
jean anyon
69
And education policy even in response to state financial challenges
has not addressed the political economy that largely determines low levels of
city district funding. Taxes on wealthy families and corporations are among
the lowest on record (Phillips, 2002). Business and government investment in
affluent suburban job centers rather than urban areas continues to deprive
poor neighborhoods of entry-level jobs and a tax base, and residents’ poverty
wages further diminish available funding sources (Anyon, 2005; Orfield, 2002;
Rusk, 1999). These political-economic constraints on quality schooling are
not challenged by current or past education policy. In most U.S. cities, the po
-
litical leverage of urban parents has not been sufficient to force the funding
necessary to overcome outdated buildings, broken computer labs, and over
-
crowded classrooms.
These economic and political conditions are the building blocks of formida
-
ble barriers to systemic, sustainable school quality. Indeed, even when urban
school reform succeeds, it fails when there are no decent jobs a diploma
from a successfully reformed school or district will attract, and there is no gov-
ernment or familial funding sufficient for the vast majority of low-income grad-
uates of even good urban high schools to obtain a bachelors degree.
Individual and neighborhood poverty builds walls around schools and class-
rooms that education policy does not penetrate or scale. In the following sec-
tion I describe some of the federal and metro-area policies and arrangements
that sustain these barriers.
Federal Policy
Analysts typically do not link federal policies to the maintenance of poverty, to
the lack of jobs that bedevils American workers, or to the increasingly large
portion of employment that pays poverty and near-poverty wages. Yet federal
policy is determinative. To take a blatant example, Congress set the first mini
-
mum wage in 1938 at $3.05 (in 2000 dollars); it stands in 2005 at $5.15 a
mere two dollars more. (Yearly income at this wage is $10,712.) This sum en
-
sures that full-time, year-round, minimum-wage work will not raise people out
of poverty (Mishel, Bernstein, & Boushey, 2003). Analysis in 2004 found that
minimum-wage standards directly affect the wages of 8.9 percent of the work
-
force (9.9 million workers); when we include those making one dollar more
an hour than the minimum wage, this legislation affects the wages of as much
as 18 percent of the workforce (Economic Policy Institute, 2004). Contrary to
the claims of those who oppose raising the minimum wage (that an increase
will force employers to fire, or hire fewer of those affected by the increase),
studies of the 1990–1991 and 1996–1997 minimum-wage increases failed to
find any systematic, significant job losses associated with the increases and
found no evidence of negative employment effects on small businesses (Eco
-
nomic Policy Institute, 2004).
Harvard Educational Review
70
Almost half the workforce earns what some economists call poverty-zone
wages (and what I define as up to and including 125% of the official poverty
level; Anyon, 2005). I analyzed figures provided by the Economic Policy Insti
-
tute to calculate the overall percentage of people who work full-time year
round yet make wages up to and including 125 percent of the official poverty
threshold needed to support a family of four at the poverty level. The analysis
demonstrates that in 1999, during a very strong economy, almost half of the
people at work in the United States (41.3%) earned poverty-zone wages — in
1999, $10.24/hour ($21,299/year) or less, working full-time year round
(Mishel et al., 2001). Two years later, in 2001, 38.4 percent earned poverty-
zone wages working full-time year round (in 2001, 125% of the poverty line
was a $10.88 hourly wage; Mishel et al., 2003). This suggests that the federal
minimum-wage policy is an important determinant of poverty for many
millions of U.S. families.
There are other macroeconomic policies that produce hardship. These es
-
pecially penalize Blacks and Latinos, the majority of whom live in segregated,
low-income urban neighborhoods. These policies include the following: job
training as a predominant federal antipoverty policy when there have been
too few jobs for graduates; ineffective federal implementation of policies that
outlaw racial discrimination in hiring and housing; regressive income taxes
that charge wealthy individuals less than half the rate charged the rich during
most of the first sixty years of the twentieth century, yet substantially raise the
payroll taxes paid by the working poor and middle class; and corporate tax
policies in recent years that allow 60 percent of large U.S. corporations to pay
no federal taxes at all (and in some cases to obtain millions in rebates; Citizens
for Tax Justice, 2002; Lafer, 2002; Orfield, 2002; Rusk, 1999).
These federal policies and practices contribute to personal, neighborhood,
and educational poverty because they lead to the following problems: There
are not enough jobs for poor families who need them; low-income families of
color are concentrated in low-resourced urban neighborhoods; and when the
wealthy do not contribute equitably to public expenses, funding for services
like education declines and the quality of the services tends to be low.
The effects of these policies are compounded by harsh union laws and lack
of federal protection for labor organizing; Federal Reserve Bank pronounce
-
ments that ignore the portion of its mandate to maintain a high level of em
-
ployment; and free-trade agreements that send thousands of corporations,
and their job opportunities, to other countries. These policies hurt workers of
all colors — and in most sectors of the economy — as existing jobs disappear
and those remaining pay lower wages, in part because they are not unionized
(Anyon, 2005; Citizens for Tax Justice, 2002; Economic Policy Institute, 2002,
2004; Galbraith, 1998; Lafer, 2002; Mishel et al., 2001).
However, there are federal policies we could create that would lower pov
-
erty by important margins — including a significantly raised minimum wage,
What “Counts” as Educational Policy?
jean anyon
71
comparable worth laws, and policies to enforce existing regulations that out
-
law discrimination in hiring. A raise in the minimum wage that brought work
-
ers above poverty would improve the lives of at least a fifth of U.S. workers
(Economic Policy Institute, 2004). Paying women the same amount men are
paid for comparable work would, according to one analysis, reduce poverty
by 40 percent, as such a large percentage of poor people are women in low-
wage jobs (Lafer, 2002). And requiring employers to hire without discriminat
-
ing against Blacks and Latinos would further open opportunities currently
denied.
In addition, policies that worked against U.S. poverty in the past could be re
-
instated: U.S. government regulation of the minimum wage, which kept low-
paid workers’ income at the median of highly paid unionized workers in the de
-
cades after World War II; federal support for union organizing; a federal pro
-
gram of job creation in cities, as during the Great Depression of the 1930s; and
federal programs for urban youth that would support further education, as
such policies did for eight million men and women after World War II (Anyon,
2005; Galbraith, 1998). These national policies were important supports of the
widespread prosperity of the United States’ working and middle classes in the
quarter century following 1945 (Galbraith, 1998).
Metropolitan Policy and Practice
Like current federal mandates, there are metro-area policies and practices
that increase the problems of urban residents and neighborhoods. Metro ar-
eas are shaped by regional markets — for jobs, housing, investment, and pro-
duction. Metro areas account for over 80 percent of national output and drive
the economic performance of the nation as a whole. Each metro area is an
-
chored by one or more cities (Dreier, Mollenkopf, & Swanstrom, 2001).
Today, metropolitan regions are characterized by population growth, ex
-
tensive inequality, and segregation (Orfield, 2002; Rusk, 1999). The percent
-
age of racial minorities in large metro areas who live in the suburbs jumped
from 19 percent to 27 percent during the 1990s. However, a growing share of
these families lives in fiscally stressed suburbs, with an increasing number of
neighborhoods having poverty levels over 30 percent (Kingsley & Petit, 2003;
Orfield, 2002). As in areas of concentrated poverty in the central city, low lev
-
els of taxable resources in these “urbanized” segregated suburbs leave services
like education lacking in funds.
U.S. metropolitan areas are characterized by the following problems, all of
which disadvantage urban minority families and communities: Most entry-
level jobs for which adults with low to moderate education levels are qualified
are increasingly located in suburbs, rather than in central cities, but public
transit systems do not connect these suburban job centers to urban areas,
where most low-income minorities live thus preventing them from access to
Harvard Educational Review
72
jobs there. State-allowed local zoning on the basis of income prevents afford
-
able housing in most suburbs where entry-level jobs are located, which means
there is little if any housing for low-income families near the suburban job
centers. Indeed, as I have mentioned, the failure to enforce antiracial discrim
-
ination statutes in housing confines most Blacks and Latinos to housing sites
in central cities and segregated suburbs. Finally, even though federal and state
taxes are paid by residents throughout metro regions (including inner cities),
most tax-supported development takes place in the affluent suburbs rather
than in low-income areas. Thus, few jobs exist in most low-income urban
neighborhoods (Anyon, 2005; Dreir et al., 2001; Orfield, 2002; Rusk, 1999).
These inequitable regional arrangements and policies exacerbate federal
wage and job mandates and contribute in important ways to joblessness and
poverty in cities and urbanized suburbs, and to the low quality of investment
in services such as education there.
Poverty
One consequence of federal and regional policies regarding work, wages,
housing segregation, and transportation is that the numbers of poor people
approach the figures of 1959 before massive urban poverty became a na-
tional issue. Although the percentages are lower now, the numbers are still
staggering: There were about as many people officially poor in 1993 (39.2
million) as in 1959 (39.4 million; Harrington, 1963). And in 2003, 35.8 mil-
lion were officially poor, only 3.5 million fewer than in 1959 (Mishel et al.,
2003).
A more realistic measure of poverty than federal guidelines is that those
earning incomes up to 200 percent of the official levels are considered poor
(Bernstein, Brocht, & Spade-Aguilar, 2000; Citro & Michael, 1995; Short, Ice
-
land, & Garner, 1999). This revised threshold is used by increasing numbers of
social scientists. A calculation of the individuals who earned less than 200 per
-
cent of the poverty level in 2001 ($17.40/hour, or $36,192/year), demon
-
strates a much larger percentage of poor employees than is commonly ac
-
knowledged: 84 percent of Hispanic workers, 80 percent of Black workers, and 64.3
percent of White workers made wages at or under 200 percent of the official poverty line
(Mishel et al., 2001).
A calculation of families living with earnings up to 200 percent of the pov
-
erty line reveals that Black and Latino families face the greatest financial hur
-
dles. More than 50 percent of Black and Latino families earn less than 200 per
-
cent of the poverty level, compared to only 20.3 percent of White families,
even though White families constitute a slight majority (50.5%) of families
that fall below 200 percent of the poverty level (Mishel et al., 2001). In sum,
poverty in the United States is higher than commonly perceived and is main
-
tained in urban areas by federal and metro-area policies and distributions.
What “Counts” as Educational Policy?
jean anyon
73
Effects of Poverty on Urban Students
Macroeconomic policies that set wages below poverty levels, that train inner-
city hopefuls for jobs that do not exist, that do not extract from the wealthy a
fair share of social expenses, and that rarely enforce laws that would substan
-
tially decrease the economic discrimination of people of color all support per
-
sistent poverty and near-poverty among minority urban populations. This eco
-
nomic and social distress can prevent children from developing their full
potential and can certainly dampen the enthusiasm, effort, and expectations
with which urban children and their families approach K–12 education.
As I will report, a recent national study of young children confirms the po
-
tential of impoverished circumstances to prevent students’ full cognitive
growth before they enroll in kindergarten. Of countervailing power, however,
is research demonstrating that when parents obtain better financial resources
or better living conditions, the educational achievement of the children typi
-
cally improves significantly. These findings empirically support the argument
that for the urban poor, even with the right educational policies in place,
school achievement may await a family’s economic access.
I already presented adult poverty figures at the official threshold and noted
the alarming increase in numbers when a more realistic assessment is made.
The same disparities exist between federal and alternative counts of poor chil-
dren. Sixteen percent of American children — almost 12 million — lived be-
low the official federal poverty line in 2001. Almost half of those children
(44%, or a little over 5 million) lived in extreme poverty (less than half the pov-
erty line, or $7,400 for a family of three in 2001) — including nearly a million
African American children. This was a 17 percent increase in the number of
children in extreme poverty from 2000, at the end of the economic boom
(Cauthen & Lu, 2001; Dillon, 2003; Lu, 2003).
When the more appropriate alternative poverty threshold criterion is ap
-
plied, however, a full 38 percent of American children are identified as poor
27 million who lived in families with income up to 200 percent of the official
poverty line. These children live in poverty as well — although official statistics
do not designate them as such. However, these families experience hardships
that are almost as severe as those who are officially poor (Cauthen & Lu 2001;
Lu, 2003). By the revised measure — 200 percent of the official poverty cutoff a full
57 percent of African American children, 64 percent of Latino, and 34 percent of White
children were poor in the United States in 2001 (Lu, 2003; Mishel et al., 2003).
It is only in the 1990s that empirical studies focused on why and how pov
-
erty affects cognitive development and school achievement. Researchers be
-
gan to document the specific effects of poverty environments on children’s
development (Brooks-Gunn, Duncan, Leventhal, & Aber, 1997; Goering &
Feins, 2003; Sampson, Morenoff, & Gannon-Rowley, 2002). This body of work
documents the correlations between low income, child development, and ed
-
ucational achievement (see Duncan & Brooks-Gunn, 1997, for an overview of
Harvard Educational Review
74
studies). For example, poverty has been found to have consistently negative
effects on children’s cognitive development (Duncan & Brooks-Gunn, 1997;
Duncan, Brooks-Gunn, & Klebanov, 1994; McLoyd, 1998). Longitudinal stud
-
ies that have been carried out also demonstrate that “family income consis
-
tently predicts children’s academic and cognitive performance, even when
other family characteristics are taken into account” (Duncan & Brooks-Gunn,
1997). Persistent and extreme poverty has been shown to be more detrimental
to children than temporary poverty (Bolger & Patterson, 1995; Duncan et al.,
1994). Family income may influence children through both lack of resources
and parental emotional stress (Bradley, 1984; McLoyd & Jartayne, 1994;
Smith, Brooks-Gunn, & Klebanov, 1997; Sugland, Zaslow, Brooks-Gunn, &
Moore, 1995). Poor children have more health and behavior difficulties than
those from more affluent families, which mitigates against educational success
(Duncan & Brooks-Gunn, 1997; Houser, Brown, & Prosser, 1997; Klerman,
1991/2003; Korenman & Miller, 1997). Studies collected by Duncan and
Brooks-Gunn teased out some of the variables within the effects of income. In
summarizing research reported in their 1997 volume Consequences of Growing
up Poor, they point out the following:
1. Income matters for the cognitive development of preschoolers “because
it is associated with the provision of a richer learning environment” (p.
601). This is true in part because family income is a “significant determi-
nant of child care environments, including center-based childcare (p.
601)....Incomeallows parents to provide their children with safer,
more stimulating home environments; to live in communities with better
schools, parks, and libraries and more challenging peers; to afford tui-
tion and other expenses associated with higher education; to purchase
or otherwise gain access to higher-quality health care; and in many other
ways to buy the things that promote the health and development of their
children” (p. 14).
2. “A variety of income measures — income [relative to needs] . . . income
loss, the ratio of debts to assets, and unstable work — are associated with
family economic pressure” (p. 602). Economic pressure has been found
to be associated with depression (and stress) in parents, which can affect
parenting, and thus school achievement.
3. “Family income is usually a stronger predictor of ability and achievement
outcomes than are measures of parental schooling or family structure
[e.g., single parenthood]” (p. 603). Many studies have shown that chil
-
dren raised in low-income families score lower than children from more
affluent families do on assessments of health, cognitive development,
and positive behavior. “In general, the better the measure of family in
-
come and the longer the period over which it is measured, the stronger
the association between the family’s economic well-being and children’s
outcomes” (p. 14).
What “Counts” as Educational Policy?
jean anyon
75
It is important to understand that these findings do not suggest that poor
students are of low intelligence; rather, the studies point to the power of the
economy — and of economic hardship — to place extremely high hurdles to
full development in front of children who are poor. It is of course possible —
although it is not the norm that education over time mitigates the effects of
SES (Hout, 1988; Jencks & Phillips, 1998).
In 2002, Valerie Lee and David Burkham published the results of a large-
sample assessment of the effects of poverty on cognitive development. They
utilized data from the United States Department of Education’s early child
-
hood longitudinal kindergarten cohort, which is a comprehensive dataset that
provides a nationally representative portrait of kindergarten students. Lee
and Burkham (2002) explored differences in young children’s achievement
scores in literacy and mathematics by race, ethnicity, and socioeconomic sta
-
tus (SES) as they began kindergarten. They also analyzed differences by social
background in an array of children’s homes and family activities.
The study demonstrates that inequalities in children’s cognitive ability by
SES are substantial even before children begin kindergarten and that poverty
has a detrimental impact on early intellectual achievement. Importantly, it
demonstrates that the disadvantages of being poor outweigh by far the race or
family structure of children as causes of the cognitive disadvantages.
Details of the national assessment include the following:
1. Before children enter kindergarten, the average cognitive scores of chil-
dren in the highest SES group are 60 percent above the scores of the low-
est SES group.
2. Cognitive skills are much less closely related to race/ethnicity after ac-
counting for SES. After taking racial differences into account, children
from different SES groups achieve at different levels before they begin
kindergarten.
3. The impact of family structure on cognitive skills (e.g., being in a single-
parent family) is much less than either race or SES.
4. Socioeconomic status is very strongly related to cognitive skills; SES ac
-
counts for more of the variation in cognitive scores than any other factor
by far.
Lee and Burkham (2002) also found that disadvantaged children not only
enter kindergarten with significantly lower cognitive skills than their advan
-
taged peers, but also that low-SES children begin school (kindergarten) in sys
-
tematically lower-quality elementary schools than their more advantaged
counterparts. “However school quality is defined in terms of higher student
achievement, more school resources, more qualified teachers, more positive
teacher attitudes, better neighborhood or school conditions, private vs. public
schools the least advantaged United States children begin their formal
schooling in consistently lower-quality schools. This reinforces the inequalities
that develop even before children reach school age” (p. 3; see also Entwistle &
Harvard Educational Review
76
Alexander, 1997; Phillips, Brooks-Gunn, Duncan, Klevanov, & Crane, 1998;
Phillips, Crouse, & Ralph, 1998; Stipic & Ryan, 1997; White, 1982).
In their review of studies of poverty’s effects on individual development,
Duncan and Brooks-Gunn (1997) conclude, “Taken together, [these studies]
suggest that programs that raise the incomes of poor families will enhance the
cognitive development of children and may improve their chance of success
in [education and] the labor market during adulthood. Most important ap
-
pears to be the elimination of deep and persistent poverty during a child’s
early years” (p. 608). I now turn to research suggesting that familial financial
and other supports do indeed lead to increased educational achievement in
children.
Evidence that Familial Supports Raise Educational Achievement
I have been examining relationships among education policy, the economy,
and achievement in urban schools. First, I critiqued education policy for its
lack of attention to urban poverty, which, I argued, is maintained by policies
and decisions made at the federal and metropolitan levels. I provided evi-
dence of some of the egregious consequences of federal and regional policies
and practices for urban families, neighborhoods, students, and schools. In
particular, I demonstrated that child poverty creates obstacles to full develop-
ment and educational achievement, especially when low-income minority
children attend low-resourced schools — which most do. In this section I pro-
vide indirect and direct research evidence that increased family supports such
as financial resources and less segregated neighborhoods raise educational
achievement.
Indirect evidence is present in a longitudinal study completed in 2003 that
found that improving family income reduces the negative (aggressive) social
behavior of children, which in turn is likely to lead to better school behavior
and performance. For eight years, researchers studied a representative popu
-
lation sample of 1,420 children ages nine to thirteen in rural North Carolina.
A quarter of the children were from a Cherokee reservation. Psychological
tests were given at the start of the study and repeated each year (Costello,
Compton, Keeler, & Angold, 2003; O’Connor, 2003).
When the study began, 68 percent of the children were living below the offi
-
cial poverty line. On average, the poorer children engaged in more vandalism,
stealing, bullying, stubbornness, and outbursts of anger than those who were
not poor. But halfway through the study, a local casino began distributing a
percentage of its profits to tribal families. Given to each tribal member over
eighteen and put in a trust fund for younger members, the payment increased
slightly each year, reaching about $6,000 per person for the year 2001. Psychi
-
atric tests administered by researchers for the four years that the funds were
being distributed demonstrated that the negative behaviors of children in
families who were no longer poor dropped to the same levels found among
What “Counts” as Educational Policy?
jean anyon
77
children whose families had never been poor (decreasing by 40%). Parents
who moved out of poverty reported having more time to spend with their chil
-
dren, and researchers identified better parenting behavior. Researchers also
identified the psychological benefits of not being poor as important to both
parents and children. Poverty puts stress on families, which can increase the
likelihood of children developing behavioral problems. One parent in the
study told researchers that “the jobs [produced by the casino] give people the
chance to pull themselves up by their bootstraps and get out of poverty. That
carries over into less juvenile crime, less domestic violence, and an overall
better living experience for families” (O’Connor, 2003, p. 2).
Other research demonstrates that urban low-income parents are also able
to practice more effective parenting strategies when some of the stress of
poverty is eased by a higher income. And the reduction in stress in turn may
positively affect the behavior and achievement of low-income children (see in
-
formation below; also Jackson, Brooks-Gunn, Huang, & Glassman, 2000; Jere
-
miah, 2003; Seitz, Rosenbaum, & Apfel, 1985).
Direct evidence that income supports improved educational achievement is
also available. In March 2001, the Manpower Demonstration Research Corpora-
tion (MDRC) published a synthesis of research on how welfare and work poli-
cies affect the children of single mothers (Morris, Huston, Duncan, Crosby, &
Bos, 2001). This synthesis reviewed data from evaluations of five programs that
provided income supplements to poverty-wage workers (Florida’s Family Tran-
sition Program, the Minnesota Family Investment Program, the National Evalu-
ation of Welfare-to-Work Strategies, Milwaukee’s New Hope for Families and
Children Program, and the Self-Sufficiency Project). These programs offered
supports of differing kinds to poverty-wage workers income supplements,
earnings disregards (rules that allow working welfare recipients to keep more of
their income when they go to work), subsidized health care, employment ser
-
vices, counseling, supervised afterschool activities for children and youth, and
informal get-togethers with project staff.
MDRC’s review of the studies found that even relatively small income sup
-
plements to working parents (amounting to about $4,000 per year) improved
children’s elementary school achievement by about 10 to 15 percent of the av
-
erage variation in the control groups. These improvements were seen on test
scores as well as on ratings by parents and/or teachers. The earning supple
-
ments had “consistently positive impacts on children’s [school] achievement”
(Morris et al., 2001, p. 63). The positive effects were small, but were statisti
-
cally significant.
Longitudinal studies have found that the achievement and behavior prob
-
lems of young children can have important implications for their well-being in
adolescence and adulthood (Caspi, Wright, Moffit, & Silva, 1998; Masten &
Coatsworth, 1995). Moreover, even small differences between children in
school achievement early on can translate into larger differences later (Ent
-
wistle & Alexander, 1997). Therefore, as the authors of the research synthesis
Harvard Educational Review
78
state, “a program’s effects on children, even if the effects are small, may con
-
tinue to have implications over the course of their lives” (Caspi et al., 1998,
p. 25).
The earning supplements provided by four of these programs did not, how
-
ever, bring the families above the poverty level. The improvements in chil
-
dren’s school achievement and behavior from even these relatively meager
cash supplements for working families suggest that if we were to increase fam
-
ily resources substantially, we could probably improve educational and social
outcomes for children substantially.
Indeed, one program that did provide an earning supplement that brought
the families above poverty level showed particularly impressive results for chil
-
dren’s behavior and achievement. New Hope for Families and Children was
run between 1994 and 1998 in two inner-city areas in Milwaukee. Candidates
had to live in one of two targeted areas, be eighteen or older, be willing and
able to work at least thirty hours per week, and have a household income at or
below 150 percent of the federal poverty level (Huston et al., 2001). Almost 90
percent of the adults in the sample were single or separated mothers with chil-
dren when they entered the study, and 80 percent were receiving public assis-
tance. The program was conceived by a nonprofit community-based organiza-
tion and provided several benefits: the earnings supplement, subsidized
health insurance, and subsidized child care. The program offered help in ob-
taining a job and provided a community-service job for up to one year for
those not able to find work elsewhere, the advice and support of project staff
were made available. The annual cost of providing these benefits was $5,300
per family.
New Hope was evaluated at two-year and five-year intervals using a random
assignment research design. After conducting outreach in the communities to
identify eligible people, the study enrolled over 1,300 low-income adults. Half
the applicants were randomly assigned to a program group that received New
Hope’s benefits, and the other half were randomly assigned to a control group
that was not eligible for the benefits.
Both evaluations showed positive results (Bos, Huston, Duncan, Brock, &
McLoyd, 1996; Huston et al., 2001). Financial supplements in the New Hope
program did reduce the number of families in poverty, but both program and
control groups reported similar levels of hardship, such as food insecurity and
financial insufficiency. Yet the program had positive effects on parents’ well-
being and coping skills. As Huston et al. (2003) explain:
Parents in the New Hope group were more aware of available “helping” re
-
sources in the community, such as where to find assistance with energy costs or
housing problems. More of them also knew about the [Earned Income Tax
Credit] and its support, an important source of support for low-income workers.
Ethnographic data suggest that a significant number of families intentionally
used the Earned Income Tax Credits as a savings plan for making major pur
-
chases, reducing debt, and stabilizing rent and other payments. Parents in New
What “Counts” as Educational Policy?
jean anyon
79
Hope also reported better physical health and fewer symptoms associated with
depression than did parents in the control group. At the two-year point, New
Hope parents reported reduced stress, increased feelings of social support, and
increased time pressure. The ethnographic study found that many parents had
children with disabilities or behavioral difficulties; New Hope helped the par
-
entsachieveadifficultbalanceamongwork,services,andparenting....The
New Hope parents did report fewer problems controlling their children, and
parents of adolescents reported more effective management (better control and
less need for punishment). (p. 9)
New Hope improved children’s school performance. “At both the two-year
and the five-year points, children in the program performed better than con
-
trol group children on several measures of academic achievement, particu
-
larly on reading and literacy tests. After five years, they scored higher on a
standardized test of reading skills and their parents reported that they got
higher grades in reading skills” (Huston et al., 2001, p. 13). These effects were
slightly more pronounced for boys than for girls. Compared with their control
group counterparts, boys in New Hope also received higher ratings of aca-
demic performance from their teachers and were more likely to expect to at-
tend college at both the two-year and the five-year assessments. “New Hope ad-
olescents reported more engagement with schools, feelings of efficacy, and
expectations to finish college than did their control group counterparts” (pp.
13–14). New Hope’s effects are consistent with the results of other programs
that have improved children’s outcomes by providing wage supplements and
subsidized child care (Michalopoulos et al., 2002; Morris et al., 2001).
Indeed, the New Hope findings are in line with the increased educational
achievement of students that has been identified in large-scale programs that
assist low-income minority families by helping them move from inner-city
neighborhoods to more affluent and/or less segregated metropolitan areas.
The first of these “mobility programs” was the Gautreaux program in the Chi
-
cago metropolitan area.
As a result of a victorious lawsuit charging the Chicago Housing Authority
with segregation in public housing, the court ordered the housing authority
to move families who wanted to live in less segregated areas of the city and sub
-
urbs. The Gautreaux program moved over 7,000 families to higher-income
areas of the Chicago metropolitan region between 1976 and 1998 (Rubinowitz
& Rosenbaum, 2002). Although at first a disproportionate number of the chil
-
dren who moved were placed in classes for the learning disabled by their sub
-
urban schools, they ultimately were significantly more likely than their urban
counterparts to be in college-bound tracks, in four-year colleges, and were
subsequently more likely to be employed in jobs with higher pay and with ben
-
efits than children who stayed in the city (Rubinowitz & Rosenbaum, 2002).
The success of the Gautreaux program led to more than fifty other mobility
programs, including the Moving To Opportunity program (MTO) begun by
Harvard Educational Review
80
the U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development (HUD) in 1994.
The Housing and Community Development Act of 1992 authorized HUD to
“assist very low-income families with children who reside in public housing or
housing receiving project-based assistance under Section 8 of the Housing
and Community Development Act of 1937 to move out of areas with high con
-
centrations of persons living in poverty (40% or more) to areas with low con
-
centrations of such persons (less than 10% in poverty)” (Goering & Feins,
2003, p. 6). Moving To Opportunity projects were carried out in five cities:
Baltimore, Boston, Chicago, Los Angeles, and New York. Congress stipulated
that HUD conduct evaluations of the program to determine its effects
(Goering & Feins, 2003).
Overall, roughly 5,300 families volunteered to move within the metropoli
-
tan area of the city in which they lived. In total, 4,608 families were eligible.
They were divided into three groups: the MTO “treatment” or experimental
group, which received Section 8 certificates or vouchers that could only be
used in areas where 10 percent or less of the residents lived below official pov-
erty levels; they also received counseling assistance in finding private rental
units. A second group was given Section 8 certificates with no special restric-
tions on where they were to move, and no counseling (Section 8–only group).
An in-place control group continued to receive housing project assistance in
the inner-city neighborhoods where they lived. The families in all three
groups of the MTO program tended to be young single mothers (under age
35), African American, with a median income of $8,200. Most stated that their
main reason for wanting to move was fear of gangs and violence in the neigh-
borhoods in which they lived.
Social scientists conducted research at all five sites, using HUD data, base-
line surveys, follow-up surveys of families, qualitative interviews, and data on
juvenile crime, labor-market outcomes, and school performance. Among
their findings are the following.
One to three years after the families in the experimental group moved, they
lived in significantly more affluent and more racially mixed communities than
families in the other two groups. In addition, those who were in the experi
-
mental group had median incomes that were 73 percent higher than the me
-
dian incomes for the control group and 53 percent higher than the Section
8–only group. In 1997, three years after the program began, the MTO experi
-
mental group families in all five metropolitan areas lived in less-segregated
neighborhoods than either of the other two groups.
Studies of adults in the experimental groups in New York and Boston re
-
ported significantly better health and emotional well-being than the Section
8–only and control groups in those cities. Mothers in both the experimental
groups were much less likely to report being depressed or stressed. The par
-
ents provided more structure for their children’s activities and used less re
-
strictive parenting styles. By the third year, 10 percent fewer of the experimen
-
What “Counts” as Educational Policy?
jean anyon
81
tal group in New York City were receiving welfare. In Boston, public assistance
for MTO families dropped by half, and employment in all MTO sites in
-
creased from 27 percent at the beginning of the program to 43 percent three
years later. Employment in Boston increased by more than one-half.
The outcomes for children in these experimental groups were also encour
-
aging. They attended schools that had higher pass rates, more affluent stu
-
dent bodies, and more resources than the schools attended by control group
children. Ludwig, Duncan, and Ladd (2003) hypothesize that the peer groups
in the new schools had more positive attitudes toward school than in the inner
city, and this may also have contributed to good outcomes for the children.
Ludwig, Duncan, and Ladd report that young children in the experimental
and Section 8–only groups “achieved higher test scores than the controls, and
experienced fewer arrests for violent criminal behavior” (2003, p. 164). The
authors report in some detail the assessments in Baltimore, and state that they
are “largely consistent with evidence from the other MTO sites” (p. 163).
Young children in the experimental and Section 8–only groups had Compre-
hensive Test of Basic Skills (CTBS) reading scores that were on average six to
seven percentage points higher than those in the control group (i.e., in low-in-
come urban schools). “This large effect is equal to around one-quarter of the
control group mean of 25 percentile points and one-quarter of a standard de-
viation in the national CTBS math distribution” (p. 165). Children in the ex-
perimental group also raised their CTBS math scores about the same amount,
and their pass rates on the Maryland Functional Tests’ (MFT) reading test
were almost double those in the inner-city schools.
High school students in the Baltimore experimental group had a more dif-
ficult transition. In the first three years of MTO, they had higher rates of grade
retention, disciplinary action, and school dropout rates than the children of
families in the other two groups. The authors suggest that these differences
may be due to the enforcement of higher behavioral and/or educational stan
-
dards in more affluent schools (Ludwig et al., 2003).
However, teens who moved from high- to low-poverty neighborhoods were
arrested less often than teens in the other groups. For example, 2.7 percent of
control group adolescents were arrested during an average three-month pe
-
riod, compared with only 1.4 percent of teens in the experimental group dur
-
ing the same period. Furthermore, there was a 50 percent reduction in the
proportion of juveniles in the experimental group who were arrested for vio
-
lent offenses. For example, in a given quarter, 3 percent of adolescents in the
control group were arrested for violent crimes, compared with only 1.4 per
-
cent among the experimental group (Ludwig et al., 2003).
Research in the Boston MTO found significantly fewer behavioral and men
-
tal health problems among boys in both the experimental and Section 8-only
groups, and experimental-group children were less likely to be injured or to
experience asthma attacks. Among children with asthma, the number of at
-
tacks requiring medical attention fell significantly (Goering & Feins, 2003).
Harvard Educational Review
82
Additionally, the children in the experimental group in Boston were less likely
to engage in antisocial behavior (Ludwig et al., 2003).
In sum, these results are in general agreement with evaluations of other mo
-
bility programs, which have generally led to “substantial improvements in . . .
neighborhood conditions, physical and mental health, safety, housing condi
-
tions, adult labor-market outcomes (although the findings here are mixed)”
(Johnson, Ladd, & Ludwig, 2002, p. 185) and improvements in the children’s
behavior and educational outcomes of families who moved.
The success of even small family supports and of a move to places of in
-
creased opportunity suggests that we should provide a financial and opportu
-
nity base for urban families. This in itself will lay the foundation for fuller
child development and educational achievement.
A New Education Policy Paradigm
I have outlined a number of federal and regional polices and practices that
undermine urban school quality and potential by maintaining large poverty
populations in urban neighborhoods. I have also provided evidence that this
poverty works against the development and achievement of urban students.
Importantly, however, we also see that even modest financial and social sup-
ports for poor families enable the children to achieve at higher levels in
school. This suggests that policies to counter the devastating effects of macro-
economic and regional mandates and practices should “count” as policies we
call on to create equity and quality in urban districts and schools.
As education policymakers and practitioners, we can acknowledge and act
on the power of urban poverty, low-wage work, and housing segregation to
dwarf most curricular, pedagogical, and other educational reforms. The ef
-
fects of macroeconomic policies continually trump the effects of education
policies.
To remove economic barriers to school quality and consequence, we can
legislate a significantly higher living wage; we can create jobs in cities that of
-
fer career ladders and prepare low-income residents to fill them. And, like a
number of European countries, we can tax wealthy families and corporations
to pay for these and other investments. We should enforce federal antidiscrim
-
ination measures to integrate segregated housing and create public transit
routes so low-income urban residents without cars are not denied access to
jobs in the suburbs. Policies like these would create a social foundation on
which high-quality schooling would rest. As has been the case in affluent sub
-
urbs, economic access creates the financial and political conditions in families
and communities for educational commitment and reward.
In this new paradigm, education policies for which we press would take on
the larger issues: Education funding reform would include the companion
need for financing neighborhood jobs and decent wages. New small schools
would be created as an important part of coordinated efforts at neighborhood
What “Counts” as Educational Policy?
jean anyon
83
revitalization for low-income residents. Vocational offerings in high school
would link to living-wage campaigns and employers who support them. Col
-
lege graduation would be understood as a continuation of government’s fi
-
nancial responsibility for public education. And lawsuits to racially integrate
districts would acknowledge housing segregation as fundamental and target
legal challenges accordingly.
Policies that set the standards schools must meet would identify the money,
materials, teachers, courses, and neighborhood needs that must be filled in
order to provide opportunities to learn at high levels. Educational account
-
ability would be conceived as a public undertaking, centrally involving fami
-
lies, communities, and students, in consultation with district and government
officials.
In this approach to urban school reform, “policy alignment” would not re
-
fer to the fit between education mandates issued by various levels of govern
-
ment and bureaucracy. The fit we would seek is between neighborhood, fam
-
ily, and student needs and the potential of education policies to contribute to
their fulfillment.
However, economic strength and political leverage is not all that is required
to transform urban education. Good schools require not only good neighbor-
hoods, but as equity-seeking educational reforms have promised also the
detracking of minority and working-class youth, a culture responsive to stu-
dents, and assistance to teachers in their struggle to surmount the wall of res-
ignation and defiance that separates many students from the educational en-
terprise.
A new paradigm of education policy is possible one that promotes equity-
seeking school change and that includes strategies to create conditions that
will allow the educational improvements to take root, grow, and bear fruit in
students’ lives.
Notes
1. The 1958 National Defense Education Act (NDEA) funded and promoted curriculum
materials, primarily in science, math, and foreign languages (e.g., the “New Math”), and
some of these probably found their way into city districts and classrooms. But the NDEA
was aimed at increasing the security and technological prowess of the United States, not
at improving urban schools.
2. Graduation rates in large urban high schools are lower than is commonly believed. Jay
P. Greene, senior fellow at the Manhattan Institute for Policy Research, calculated grad
-
uation rates in all states and large cities for major racial groups. For this calculation he
first identified the eighth-grade public school enrollment for each jurisdiction and for
each subgroup from the 1993 fall semester, adjusting for student movement into or out
of an area. He then obtained counts of the number of regular high school diplomas
awarded in the spring of 1998 when the eighth graders should have been graduating.
(In calculating the 1998 graduation rate, he did not include later GED or other alterna
-
tive diplomas, as the federal government does.) He found that the national graduation
rate for the class of 1998 was 71 percent. For White students the rate was 78 percent, for
Harvard Educational Review
84
African American students it was 56 percent, and for Latinos, 54 percent. In fifteen of
forty-five large (mostly urban) districts for which there were data, fewer than 50 percent
of African American students graduated; and in twenty-one of thirty-six large, mostly ur
-
ban districts for which there were data, fewer than 50 percent of Latino students gradu
-
ated (Greene, 2001, pp. 1–5).
References
Anyon, J. (1979). Ideology and U.S. history textbooks. Harvard Educational Review, 49,
361–386.
Anyon, J. (1980). Social class and the hidden curriculum of work. Journal of Education,
162, 7–92.
Anyon, J. (1981). Social class and school knowledge. Curriculum Inquiry, 11, 3–42.
Anyon, J. (1995). Race, social class, and educational reform in an inner city school. Teach
-
ers College Record, 97, 69–94.
Anyon, J. (1997). Ghetto schooling: A political economy of urban educational reform. New York:
Teachers College Press.
Anyon, J. (in press). Radical possibilities: Public policy, urban education, and a new social move
-
ment. New York: Routledge.
Ayres, L. (1909). Laggards in our schools: A study of retardation and elimination in city school sys
-
tems. New York: Russell Sage.
Bernstein, J., Brocht, C., & Spade-Aguilar, M. (2000). How much is enough? Basic family bud-
gets for working families. Washington, DC: Economic Policy Institute.
Bolger, K., & Patterson, C. (1995). Psychosocial adjustment among children experiencing
persistent and intermittent family economic hardship. Child Development, 66,
1107–1129.
Bradley, R. (1984). One hundred, seventy-four children: A study of the relation between
the home environment and early cognitive development in the first 5 years. In A. Gott-
fried (Ed.), The home environment and early cognitive development (pp. 5–56). Orlando, FL:
Academic Press.
Brooks-Gunn, J., Duncan, G., Leventhal, T., & Aber, L. (1997). Lessons learned and fu-
ture directions for research on the neighborhoods in which children live. In J. Brooks-
Gunn, G. Duncan, & L. Aber (Eds.), Neighborhood poverty, volume 1: Contexts and conse
-
quences for children (pp. 279–298). New York: Russell Sage.
Bos, J., Huston, A. C., Duncan, G .J., Brock, T., & McLoyd, V. (1996). New hope for people
with low incomes: Two-year results of a program to reduce poverty and reform welfare. New York:
Manpower Demonstration Research Corporation.
Caspi, A., Wright, B., Moffit, E., & Silva, T. (1998). Early failure in the labor market: Child
-
hood and adolescent predictors of unemployment in the transition to adulthood.
American Sociological Review, 63, 424–451.
Cauthen, N., & Lu, H. (2001, August). Living on the edge: Employment alone is not enough for
America’s low-income children and families (Research Brief No. 1, Mailman School of Pub
-
lic Health, National Center for Children in Poverty). New York: Columbia University.
Citizens for Tax Justice. (2002). Surge in corporate tax welfare drives corporate tax payments
down to near record low. Washington, DC: Author.
Citro, C., & Michael, R. (Eds.). (1995). Measuring poverty: A new approach. Washington, DC:
National Academy Press.
Conley, D. (2003). Who governs our schools? Changing roles and responsibilities. New York:
Teachers College Press.
Costello, J., Compton, S., Keeler, G., & Angold, A. (2003). Relationships between poverty
and psychopathology: A natural experiment. Journal of the American Medical Association,
290, 2023–2029.
Cross, C. (2004). Political education: National policy comes of age. New York: Teachers College
Press.
What “Counts” as Educational Policy?
jean anyon
85
Dillon, S. (2003, April 30). Report finds number of black children in deep poverty rising.
New York Times, p. 18A.
Dreier, P., Mollenkopf, J., & Swanstrom, T. (2001). Place matters: Metropolitics for the 21st cen
-
tury. Lawrence: University Press of Kansas.
Duncan, G., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (Eds.). (1997). Consequences of growing up poor. New York:
Russell Sage.
Duncan, G., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Klebanov, P. (1994). Economic deprivation and early
childhood development. Child Development, 65, 296–318.
Economic Policy Institute. (2002). Economic snapshots. Washington, DC: Author.
Economic Policy Institute. (2004). EPI issue guide: Minimum wage. Washington, DC: Author.
Education Trust. (2001). The funding gap: Low-income and minority students receive fewer dol
-
lars. Washington, DC: Author.
Entwistle, D., & Alexander, K. (1997). Children, schools, and inequality. Boulder, CO: West
-
view Press.
Fine, M. (2001, May). Comparative analysis of the organization of high schools 1996–97, NYC
Board of Education. Findings presented at the Spencer Conference, New York. Docu
-
ment available at www.nysed.gov.80/emsc/docs4-99NYStrategy.ppt.3.
Galbraith, J. (1998). Created unequal: The crisis in American pay. New York: Free Press.
Goering, J., & Feins, J. (Eds.). (2003). Choosing a better life? Evaluating the Moving To Oppor
-
tunity social experiment. Washington, DC: Urban Institute Press.
Greene, J. (2001). High school graduation rates in the United States. Washington, DC: Black
Alliance for Educational Options and the Manhattan Institute.
Harrington, M. (1963). The other America: Poverty in the United States. Baltimore: Penguin.
Houser, R. M., Brown, B. V., & Prosser, W. R. (1998). Indicators of children’s well-being. New
York: Russell Sage.
Hout, M. (1988). More universalism, less structural mobility: The American occupational
structure in the 1980s. American Journal of Sociology, 93, 1358–1400.
Huston, A. C., Duncan, G. J., Granger, R., Bos, J., McLoyd, V. C., Mistry, R., Crosby, D. A.,
Gibson, C., Magnuson, K., Romich, J., & Ventura, A. (2001). Work-based anti-poverty
programs for parents can enhance the school performance and social behavior of chil-
dren. Child Development, 72, 318–336.
Huston, A. C., Miller, C., Richburg-Hayes, L., Duncan, G. J., Eldred, C. A., Weisner, T. S.,
Lowe, E., McLoyd, V. C., Crosby, D. A., Ripke, M. N., & Redcross, C. (2003). Summary re
-
port, New Hope for families and children: Five-year results of a program to reduce poverty and re
-
form welfare. New York: Manpower Demonstration Research Corporation.
Jackson, A., Brooks-Gunn, J., Huang, C., & Glassman, M. (2000) Single mothers in low-
wage jobs: Financial strain, parenting, and preschoolers’ outcomes. Child Development
71, 1409–1423.
Jencks, C., & Phillips, M. (1998) The Black/White test score gap. Washington, DC: Brookings
Institution Press.
Jeremiah, L. (2003). Family support programs and academic achievement: Lessons for Seattle. Un
-
published manuscript. Available online at http://www.evans.washington.edu/research/
psclinic/pdf/02-03dp/Jeremiahdp.pdf.
Johnson, M., Ladd, H., & Ludwig, J. (2002). The benefits and costs of residential mobility
programs. Housing Studies 17, 125–138.
Kingsley, T., & Petit, K. (2003). Concentrated poverty? A change in course. Neighborhood
change in urban America series. Washington, DC: Urban Institute.
Klerman, L. (1991; 2003 Reprint edition). The health of poor children: Problems and
programs. In A. C. Huston (Ed.), Children and poverty: Child development and public policy
(pp. 136–157). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Korenman, S., & Miller, J. (1997). Effects of long-term poverty on physical health of chil
-
dren in the national longitudinal survey of youth. In G. Duncan & J. Brooks-Gunn
(Eds.), Consequences of growing up poor (pp. 70–99). New York: Russell Sage.
Lafer, G. (2002). The job training charade. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Harvard Educational Review
86
Lee, V., & Burkham, D. (2002). Inequality at the starting gate: Social background and achieve
-
ment at kindergarten entry. Washington, DC: Economic Policy Institute.
Lu, H. (2003). Low-income children in the United States. New York: Columbia University, Mail
-
man School of Public Health.
Ludwig, J., Duncan, G., & Ladd, H. (2003). The effects of moving to opportunity on chil
-
dren and parents in Baltimore. In J. Goering & J. Feins (Eds.), Choosing a better life? (pp.
153–177). Washington, DC: Urban Institute Press.
Masten, A., & Coatsworth, D. (1995). The structure and coherence of competence from
childhood through adolescence. Child Development, 66, 1635–1659.
McLoyd, V. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psy
-
chologist, 53, 185–204.
McLoyd, V., & Jartayne, T. (1994). Unemployment and work interruption among African-
American single mothers: Effects on parenting and adolescent socio-emotional func
-
tioning. Child Development, 65, 562–589.
Miao, J., & Haney, W. (2004). High school graduation rates: Alternative methods and im
-
plications. Education Policy Analysis Archives, 12(55). Available online at http://
epaa.asu.edu/epaa/v12n55.
Michaloupolos, C., Tattri, D., Miller, C., Robins, P. K., Morris, P., Gyarmati, D., Redcross, C.,
Foley, K., & Ford, R. (2002). Making work pay: Final report on the self-sufficiency project for
long-term welfare recipients. New York: Manpower Demonstration Research Corporation.
Mishel, L., Bernstein, J., & Boushey, H. (2003). The state of working America: 2002/2003.
Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Mishel, L., Bernstein, J., & Schmitt, J. (2001). The state of working America: 2000/2001.
Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Morris, P., Huston, A.C., Duncan, G.J., Crosby, D., & Bos, J. (2001). How welfare and work
policies affect children: A synthesis of research. Washington, DC: Manpower Demonstration
Research Corporation.
O’Connor, A. (2003, October 21). Rise in income improves children’s behavior. New York
Times, p. F5.
Odden, A., & Picus, L. (1992). School finance: A policy perspective. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Orfield, M. (2002). American metropolitics: The new suburban reality. Washington, DC:
Brookings Institute.
Phillips, K. (2002). Wealth and democracy: A political history of the American rich. New York:
Broadway Books.
Phillips, M., Brooks-Gunn, J., Duncan, G., Klevanov, P., & Crane, J. (1998). Family back
-
ground, parenting practices, and the Black/White test score gap. In C. Jencks & M.
Phillips (Eds.), The Black/White test score gap (pp. 103–145). Washington, DC: Brookings
Institution Press.
Phillips, M., Crouse, J., & Ralph, J. (1998). Does the Black/White test score gap widen af
-
ter children enter school? In C. Jencks & M. Phillips (Eds.), The Black/White test score gap
(pp. 229–272). Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.
Ravitch, D. (2000). The great school wars: A history of the New York City public schools. Balti
-
more: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Rubinowitz, L., & Rosenbaum, J. (2002). Crossing the class and color line: From public housing
to White suburbia. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Rusk, D. (1999). Inside game/outside game: Winning strategies for saving urban America
. Wash
-
ington, DC: Brookings Institution.
Sampson, R., Morenoff, J., & Gannon-Rowley, T. (2002). Assessing “neighborhood ef
-
fects:” Social processes and new directions in research. Annual Review of Sociology, 28,
443–478.
Seitz, V., Rosenbaum L., & Apfel, N. (1985). Effects of family support intervention: A ten-
year follow-up. Child Development 56, 376–391.
Short, K., Iceland, J., & Garner, T. (1999). Experimental poverty measures: 1998. Washington,
DC: U.S. Census Bureau.
What “Counts” as Educational Policy?
jean anyon
87
Smith, J., Brooks-Gunn, J., and Klebanov, P. (1997). Consequences of living in poverty for
young children’s cognitive and verbal ability and early school achievement. In G.
Duncan & J. Brooks-Gunn (Eds.), Consequencs of growing up poor (pp. 132–189). New
York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Stein, S. (2004). The culture of educational policy. New York: Teachers College Press.
Stipic, D., & Ryan, R. (1997). Economically disadvantaged preschoolers: Ready to learn
but further to go. Developmental Psychology, 33, 711–723.
Sugland, B., Zaslow, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1995). The early childhood HOME inventory
and HOME short form in differing socio-cultural groups: Are there differences in un
-
derlying structure, internal consistency of subcases, and patterns of prediction? Jour
-
nal of Family Issues, 16, 632–663.
Tyack, D. (1974). The one best system: A history of American urban education. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
White, K. (1982). The relationship between socioeconomic status and academic achieve
-
ment. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 46–81.
Wrigley, J. (1982). Class politics and public schools: Chicago 1900–1950. New Brunswick, NJ:
Rutgers University Press.
I would like to thank my colleague Tony Picciano for his thoughts.
Harvard Educational Review
88
This article has been reprinted with permission of the Harvard Educational Review (ISSN
0017-8055) for personal use only. Posting on a public website or on a listserv is not allowed.
Any other use, print or electronic, will require written permission from the Review. You may
subscribe to HER at www.harvardeducationalreview.org. HER is published quarterly by the
Harvard Education Publishing Group, 8 Story Street, Cambridge, MA 02138, tel. 617-495-
3432. Copyright © by the President and Fellows of Harvard College. All rights reserved.