Top Lang Disorders
Vol. 36, No. 3, pp. 198–216
Copyright
c
2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.
Words Are Not Enough
Providing the Context for Social
Communication and Interaction
Pamela Rosenthal Rollins
This article elucidates the unfolding of 3 phases of cognitive development through which typical
children move during the first 2 years of life to illuminate the interrelationships among early cogni-
tion, communicative intention, and word-learning strategies. The resulting theoretical framework
makes clear the developmental prerequisites for social communication and sheds light on how
some children with autism spectrum disorder can learn words and phrases but fail to develop true
social language. This framework is then applied to a case example of a child called Henry, using
data from 10-min videos of clinician–child interaction that were collected each week to evaluate
the child’s progress in social communication while working with his graduate-student clinician.
Eye-tracking data also were collected as an indirect measure of eye contact. The data showed
that Henry made progress in social engagement, reciprocal verbal interactions, and diversity of
communicative intentions. In addition, eye-tracking data suggested an increase in eye contact
commensurate with a typical age mate. Implications for social communication intervention are
discussed. Key words: autism spectrum disorders, emerging language, eye tracking, social
cognition
A
UTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER (ASD) is a
heterogeneous neurodevelopmental dis-
order that severely compromises the develop-
ment of social relatedness, reciprocity, social
communication, joint attention, and learning.
The Centers for Disease Control and Preven-
tion (2014) estimated that 1 in 68 children
Author Affiliation: School of Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, The Callier Center for Communication
Disorders, University of Texas at Dallas.
The author thank the children and families who par-
ticipated in this study, the preschool program staff for
allowing her into their program, Dr Julia Evans for the
use of equipment, Megan Nauta, Mary Grace Shafer,
Allison Kroiss, and Shreya Krishnan for assistance with
training the student clinician and coding the data, and
Dr Sharon Lynn Bear for her editorial assistants.
The author has indicated that she has no financial dis-
closures to report. She has a nonfinancial relationship
with the owners of Pathways Early Autism Interven-
tion.
Corresponding Author: Pamela Rosenthal Rollins,
EdD, School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, The
Callier Center for Communication Disorders, Univer-
sity of Texas at Dallas, 1966 Inwood Rd, Dallas, TX
DOI: 10.1097/TLD.0000000000000095
are on the autism spectrum. Although there is
no cure for ASD, early identification and inter-
vention can make a significant difference in a
child’s cognitive, language, and adaptive func-
tioning (Dawson et al., 2010; Reichow, 2012;
Wallace & Rogers, 2010; Warren et al., 2011).
Deficits in social communication and interac-
tion, however, persist throughout life. This
makes understanding the early interrelation-
ship between social-cognitive challenges and
language development critical for supporting
the long-term success of these children.
IMPORTANCE OF SHARED ATTENTION
It is well recognized that children with ASD
have extraordinary challenges in develop-
ing shared attention and intention (Charman,
2003; Mundy, Sigman, & Kasari, 1990; Rollins,
1999; Rollins & Snow, 1998). Sharing at-
tention and intention is often demonstrated
when the child directs another’s attention
toward objects or events for the purpose
of sharing and when the child coordinates
attention between a social partner and ob-
jects or events of mutual interest. This
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
198
Words Are Not Enough 199
important social-cognitive milestone marks
the emergence of the intentional stance,
or the u nderstanding that other people have
attentions and intentions that are different
from their own, which can be shared verbally
or nonverbally with their communicative part-
ner (Rollins & Snow, 1998; Tomasello, 1999;
Tomasello & Carpenter, 2007). The emer-
gence of shared attention and intention sig-
nifies true social understanding and provides
the infrastructure for social communication
(Rollins, 2003; Tomasello, Carpenter, Call,
Behne, & Moll, 2005) and structural lan-
guage (Gillespie-Lynch et al., 2013; Rollins
& Snow, 1998). Typical infants routinely en-
gage in shared attention with their caregivers
by 12 months (Bakeman & Adamson, 1984;
Carpenter, Nagel, & Tomasello, 1998), the age
at which first words often appear.
LINGUISTICALLY BASED
INTERVENTIONS AND SHARED
ATTENTION
Unlike typical children, children with ASD
often acquire words and phrases in the ab-
sence of shared attention (Rollins, 1999). This
apparent disassociation between language
and social cognition may make children with
ASD appear linguistically more sophisticated
than they actually are. Although there are
exceptions (see Kasari, Paparella, Freeman,
& Jahromi, 2008), language interventions
too often focus on language form without
considering the child’s underlying social-
cognitive skills, which are necessary for the
development of social communication. Many
language interventionists use evidence-based
practices, such as naturalistic interventions
that follow the child’s focus of attention, to
model language or expand on the language
forms produced by emerging-language chil-
dren with ASD. These linguistically based in-
terventions are rooted in language acquisition
and intervention research that is conducted
with participants who already are c apable
of shared attention and intention (Rollins,
2003, 2014). Such linguistically based inter-
ventions are developmentally too advanced
for children with ASD who use words even
though they have not yet established shared
attention for the intersubjective purpose
of sharing objects or events with another
person.
When exposed to linguistically based in-
terventions, children with ASD may develop
large vocabularies but fail to integrate their
word knowledge into generative language. In
addition, they may use isolated words and
phrases to request and label, which falls short
of the depth of true social communication.
True social communication would involve di-
recting another’s attention, such as by point-
ing and showing or commenting to share in-
formation. This requires the understanding
that others have attentions and intentions that
are different from one’s own (Camaioni, 1993;
Rollins & Snow, 1998). Intervention priorities
for children with ASD who are beginning to
use words should not focus solely on periph-
eral linguistic forms. Examples of linguistic
form goals include the acquisition of nouns
(e.g., body parts and object labels), increas-
ing the mean length of utterances by requiring
the child to use modifier-plus-noun sentence
constructions (e.g., labeling red ball vs. green
ball), or producing frozen phrases (e.g., I want
______, please). Rather, goals must be located
within a developmental framework appropri-
ate to the child’s social-cognitive abilities.
The purpose of this article was to eluci-
date the unfolding of the three phases of
cognitive development through which typi-
cal children move during the first 2 years of
life and to illuminate the interrelationships
among early cognition, communicative inten-
tion, and word-learning strategies (Figure 1).
The resulting theoretical framework makes
clear the developmental prerequisites for so-
cial communication and sheds light on how
some children with ASD can learn words and
phrases but fail to develop true social lan-
guage. These ideas are then translated into
clinical recommendations and illustrated with
a clinical case example.
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
It is well accepted that infants are socially
motivated and prone to orient themselves
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
200 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2016
Figure 1. The interrelationships among early cognition, communication, word learning strategies, and
language (adapted from Rollins, 2014), of which only Phase 1 is the developmental precursor for social
cognition communication and interaction.
toward socially salient information from
very early in life. Newborns prefer lis-
tening to speech over nonspeech sounds
(Vouloumanos, Hauser, Werker, & Martin,
2010), and they are able to imitate adults of
their own volition (Meltzoff & Moore, 1983,
1989). They engage in vocal turn-taking in-
teractions (Trevarthen, 1979), respond differ-
entially to persons and objects (Legerstee,
1991; Trevarthen, 1979), and selectively at-
tend to human faces (Johnson & Morton,
1991; Maurer, 1985). Over the first years of
life, typical infants and young children un-
dergo several qualitative changes in how they
monitor, control, and predict the behavior
of others, culminating in the capacity for
mutual understanding and cooperation with
people around them. These gradual qualita-
tive changes have been quantified as move-
ment from “sharing emotions” to “sharing
perceptions and pursuing goals” to “sharing
attention and intention” (Tomasello et al.,
2005). As will be made clear, sharing emotions
and sharing attention and intention are so-
cially motivated whereas sharing perceptions
and pursuing goals are not. In fact, sharing
emotions is the developmental precursor to
sharing attention and intention (Adamson &
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Words Are Not Enough 201
Russell, 1999; Rochat & Striano, 1999; Rollins
& Greenwald, 2013; Stern, 1985; Tomasello
et al., 2005) and, as such, constitutes the cog-
nitive underpinning for social communication
(Figure 1).
Phase 1: Sharing emotions
Around 2 months of age, infants across
cultures become increasingly alert and begin
to smile in response to social stimuli (Spitz,
1965; Wolff, 1987). The onset of social smil-
ing, coupled with an increase in gazing at
the caregiver’s face, is highly significant to
Western culture, as it launches dyads into a
new quality of shared experiences (Rochat
& Striano, 1999; Stern, 1985). These dyadic,
face-to-face interactions reflect well-balanced,
reciprocal, and rhythmic exchanges of af-
fect and emotions (Brazelton, Koslowski, &
Main, 1974; Stern, 1985; Trevarthen, 1979)
and are a precursor to later shared attention
(Adamson & Russell, 1999; Rochat & Striano,
1999; Rollins & Greenwald, 2013; Stern, 1985;
Tomasello et al., 2005). Within these inter-
actions, sensitive caregivers respond to the
infant as a communicative partner, and the
exchanges take on a conversational quality
(Snow, 1977), so much so that they have been
referred to as “protoconversations” (Bateson,
1975; Trevarthen, 1979).
Phase 2: Sharing perceptions and
pursuing goals
Around 6 months, typically developing
children become more interested in objects
and the interaction turns from dyadic to
triadic. Triadic refers to the child, the care-
giver, and a third entity, such as an object or
event. As illustrated in Figure 1, the cognitive
capabilities developed during this phase of
sharing perceptions and pursuing goals and
the emanating behavior and linguistic skills
are not inherently social (Tomasello et al.,
2005). What makes children appear to be
social during this phase is the caregiver’s
contribution to the interaction. For example,
caregivers, especially in Western cultures, fo-
cus on what the child is playing with and may
actively follow the child’s focus of attention
(e.g., by commenting on the child’s play with
objects). In so doing, the caregiver creates the
appearance of a shared interaction. Bakeman
and Adamson (1984) described these early
triadic interactions as “passive joint engage-
ment.” Children’s roles are passive in these
early interactions because they do not ac-
knowledge the caregiver’s contribution to the
interaction by attending to both the caregiver
and a shared referent. Rather, the caregiver
actively supports the child’s perceptions
by expanding the child’s solitary focus to
include the caregiver’s verbal and nonverbal
information about the attentional target.
Understanding another’s action and
predicting what comes next
The phase of sharing perceptions and pur-
suing goals can be challenging to understand.
The nuanced behaviors of which typical in-
fants are capable during this phase of cogni-
tion also may be assigned to children with
ASD. This contributes to misconceptions or
misunderstandings about the interactional ca-
pabilities of these children, whose cognitive
skills actually may have plateaued in this
phase. During this phase, both typical infants
and children with ASD engage in triadic in-
teractions that are not socially motivated, but
the interactions do help them acquire many
important cognitive abilities. Specifically, c hil-
dren in the phase of sharing perceptions and
pursuing goals are goal directed. They have
selective attention to their goals and often per-
sist until their goals are met.
In addition, they begin to understand that
their caregivers have intentional actions. They
monitor their caregiver’s actions and can
make predictions about what action comes
next in the interaction exchange, allowing
them to take turns with objects. For example,
the caregiver and the infant between 6 and
9 months of age may both be looking at
a block. The caregiver may start to build a
block tower. The infant learns to predict what
comes next and joins in an alternating se-
quence of placing blocks on the tower. After
the caregiver places a block on the tower, the
infant predicts that it is his or her turn and
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
202 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2016
does the same. Although they are both per-
ceiving the blocks and experiencing the same
activity of building the tower, the infant does
not yet look back at the caregiver and coor-
dinate his or her attention between the block
and the adult. The infant does not yet have
the understanding that they are sharing their
attention and intention to build the tower.
Regulating behavior
Toward the end of the phase for sharing
perceptions and pursuing goals (around 9–
10 months), typically developing infants
begin to use gestures and vocalizations
with communicative intent (Bates, Benigni,
Bretherton, Camaioni, & Volterra, 1979;
Bates, Camaioni, & Volterra, 1975). The
earliest preverbal intentions serve a pro-
toimperative function (i.e., nonverbal forms
of requesting and protesting). Typically
developing infants who have the cognitive
ability of sharing perceptions and pursuing
goals are able to regulate or influence the
behavior of others but may not be able to in-
fluence their mental states (Camaioni, 1993).
Influencing another’s behavior requires little
more than the attribution of agency to the
interactive partner and the ability to share
perceptions of the intended goal (Camaioni,
1993; Tomasello et al., 2005). Children in this
phase have the will to affect the caregiver
by some purposeful behavior (Ninio & Snow,
1996), using instrumental communicative
acts such as requesting (actions, objects, or
assistance) and protesting.
Phase 3: Sharing attention and intention
As typical infants make the transition to
the phase of sharing attention and intentions
(between 10 and 12 months), they are ca-
pable not only of monitoring the caregiver’s
behavior but also of actively monitoring
the caregiver’s attentional focus (Bakeman &
Adamson, 1984; Carpenter et al., 1998). This
milestone is sometimes referred to as “re-
sponding to joint attention” (Mundy & Thorp,
2008). It marks the infant’s recognition that
the caregiver’s attention is different from his
or her own (Tomasello, 1995; Trevarthen &
Hubley, 1978). Soon, the infant begins to co-
ordinate his or her attention between the
caregiver and an object of mutual interest
and is actively looking back and forth be-
tween the caregiver and the object of atten-
tion (Bakeman & Adamson, 1984; Carpenter
et al., 1998).
This newly acquired social competency is a
form of cooperative intersubjectivity, as it in-
cludes the active sharing of thoughts and emo-
tions about an outside entity (Trevarthen &
Aitken, 2001). Tomasello et al. (2005) referred
to this development as “shared intention,” as it
reflects the understanding that other persons
have unique objects of attention and inten-
tions. This new level of social-cognitive skill
emerges around the first birthday in typical
children, but it is extraordinarily difficult for
children with ASD to attain (Camaioni, 1993;
Rollins & Snow, 1998; Rollins, Wambacq,
Dowell, Mathews, & Reese, 1998; Tomasello
et al., 2005).
The development of shared attention and
intention requires that both the adult and the
child have mutual knowledge that they are do-
ing something together in relationship, which
marks the emergence of mutual cooperation
(Tomasello et al., 2005). When the preverbal
child is capable of understanding that oth-
ers have attentions and intentions different
from his or her own, true social communica-
tion emerges (Camaioni, 1993). Children with
the social-cognitive understanding of shared
intention are capable of directing the care-
giver’s attention with gestures by showing an
object or pointing to an object for the pur-
pose of sharing interest (Bates et al., 1975;
Ninio & Snow, 1996; Tomasello, Carpenter,
& Liszkowski, 2007; Wetherby, Yonclas, &
Bryan, 1989).
As young children learn words, their e arly
communicative repertoire continues to re-
flect the unfolding of shared intentionality
and the mutual understanding that they are
communicating with another. Directing the
other’s attention (which has been referred to
as “initiating joint attention”; see Mundy &
Thorp, 2008) and a new skill of discussing
a joint focus of attention continue into the
second year of life. These early discussions
of the here and now often take the form of
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Words Are Not Enough 203
commenting on objects or events in the imme-
diate environment while interacting around
toys or looking at picture books together.
TYPICAL WORD LEARNING
Children use a combination of perceptual,
cognitive, social, and linguistic inputs or cues
for word learning, and the child’s sensitiv-
ity to these cues changes over the course
of development (Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff, &
Hollich, 2000; Hollich et al., 2000; Rollins,
2003). Across the early word-learning period
(under 2 years), typically developing children
use both perceptual and social cues to iden-
tify words and meaning relations. Perceptual
cues, such as those that pair interesting things
they see with what they hear, dominate early
in development (i.e., before 10 months of
age). A progressive shift occurs toward the re-
liance on social factors around 12–18 months
(Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff, Hennon, & Maguire,
2004; Hollich et al., 2000; Rollins, 2003). This
means that typical infants and children with
ASD who are functioning in the phase of shar-
ing perceptions and pursuing goals may be
learning words by using the earlier mecha-
nism of temporal pairing of what they hear
with perceptually salient objects and events
they see (Hirsh-Pasek et al., 2000; Rollins,
2003; Rollins & Trautman, 2011). In contrast,
children with the ability to share attention
and intention begin to rely more heavily on
social cues and what appears to be intended.
These children learn new words in situations
in which the adult looks at and labels an ob-
ject that the child is not looking at. Here, for
word learning to occur, the child must use
eye contact and gaze shifts to determine the
adult’s intended focus (Baldwin, 1993). The
child also can learn the names of objects that
the adult intends (Tomasello & Barton, 1994).
HOW ASD IS DIFFERENT
Words in ASD
The apparent dissociation between lan-
guage and social cognition in children with
ASD may be explained through the lens of
the word learning and communicative func-
tions available to children in the second
phase of sharing perceptions and pursuing
goals. Unlike typical infants who pass through
this phase before they begin to talk (6–
10 months), many children with ASD exhibit
sharing perceptions and pursuing goals capa-
bilities in early childhood (3–5 years) or even
later. These children with ASD may acquire
many words through the early word-learning
strategy of pairing perceptually salient objects
and events with the words and phrases that
they hear. Caregivers, teachers, and therapists
then support the child’s perceptions of ob-
jects and events through naming the object or
event. This, in turn, may facilitate the acquisi-
tion of many words (Rollins, 2003; Tomasello,
1999), but it does not lead to rule-governed
generative language (Tomasello, 1999). True
integration of vocabulary into grammar does
not happen until a child is able to share atten-
tion and intention (Bates & Goodman, 1997,
1999; Rollins, 2003; Rollins & Snow, 1998;
Tomasello, 1999).
Requesting and labeling in ASD
Consistent with the repertoire of commu-
nicative behaviors that can be acquired during
the phase of sharing perceptions and pursuing
goals (as shown in Figure 1), many children
with ASD learn to use words and phrases for
the purpose of regulating or controlling an-
other person’s behavior (i.e., requesting and
protesting) but not for drawing their atten-
tion to objects or events of mutual interest
and sharing information. Some children with
ASD may begin to label objects and events in
their environment without any evidence that
they are sharing information with their social
partner.
Labeling is often elusive and misunder-
stood because the adult’s support of the
child’s attention gives the appearance of a
shared interaction. Labels appear more self-
directed and lack the intention to share in-
formation with another person. Labeling is
distinct from commenting, which denotes a
shared mutual understanding with another
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
204 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2016
person (Rollins, 2014). Some evidence-based
intervention practices require children with
ASD who are beginning to use words to la-
bel pictures and objects to use progressively
longer utterances for this function. These lin-
guistically based interventions, however, do
not provide the social environment necessary
for social communication to develop, as they
enable the child to continue to practice and
use forms that are not socially motivated. In
contrast, there is evidence to suggest that,
before a child with ASD develops shared at-
tention and intention, his or her interven-
tion should focus on critical foundational so-
cial communication skills, such as face-to-
face reciprocal social interactions (Ingersoll &
Gergans, 2007; Rollins, Campbell, Hoffman, &
Self, 2016; Schertz & Odom, 2007; Wallace &
Rogers, 2010), that are precursors to shared at-
tention (Adamson & Russell, 1999; Greenspan
& Shanker, 2004; Rollins et al., 2016; Rollins
& Greenwald, 2013).
Clinical recommendation
For social communication and creative lan-
guage to develop in a child, intervention prior-
ities must be located within a developmental
framework that pertains to the child’s social-
cognitive functioning. As described earlier
and illustrated in Figure 1, the first phase of
sharing emotions is the developmental pre-
cursor to the third phase of sharing attention
and intention. This three-phase framework
guides interventionists who work with chil-
dren on the spectrum who are functioning in
the second phase of sharing perceptions and
pursuing goals to focus their intervention ef-
forts instead on facilitating shared emotional
skills of face-to-face reciprocal social interac-
tions, mutual eye gazing, and contingent imi-
tation of vocalizations and gestures.
A handful of interventions are now available
that are mindful of the child’s social-cognitive
level and that address the early foundational
social communication skills necessary for
shared attention and creative language (see
Ingersoll & Gergans, 2007; Kasari et al., 2008;
Rollins et al., 2016; Schertz & Odom, 2007).
These interventions provide the social envi-
ronment necessary for social communication
by facilitating dyadic, face-to-face reciprocal
social interactions. In particular, our clinical
recommendation is for interventionists who
currently work with children in the second
phase of sharing perceptions and pursuing
goals to facilitate Phase 1 sharing emotions
skills by including five key features essential
for this phase. The five key features include
the following: (a) position yourself face to
face with the child; (b) engage in social
sensory routines, limiting the use of toys
whenever possible; (c) establish eye contact
without verbal and physical prompts, making
sure to reinforce eye contact immediately;
(d) use animation; and (e) use contingent
imitation of the child’s gestures, vocalization,
and words. To support this recommendation,
I provide a clinical case example of Henry
and his graduate-student clinician, Alice.
CLINICAL CASE EXAMPLE
Participants
Henry, a 29-month-old toddler with ASD
(see Table 1 for test scores and demographic
information), and Alice, his graduate-student
clinician, participated in this project. Henry
was functioning within the second cognitive
phase of sharing perceptions and pursuing
goals. Consistent with a child in this phase,
Henry used verbal and nonverbal means to
request and label items in his environment.
Henry, however, rarely looked at his commu-
nicative partner; instead, he focused his gaze
on the objects around him.
Henry was enrolled in a linguistically based
program for toddlers aged 18 months to 3
1
/
2
years with ASD or other developmental disor-
ders. The program is a training site for grad-
uate students in speech–language pathology
and provides intensive communication inter-
ventions 4 days a week for 2
1
/
2
hr. Graduate
students provide the children with both
group and individual therapies. In this linguis-
tically based program, the goals for toddlers
with ASD who function in the second phase
of sharing perceptions and pursuing goals
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Words Are Not Enough 205
Table 1. Henry’s test scores and family
demographics
Measure Result
Henry’s age at entry 29 months
Mother’s age 35 years
Mother’s education Bachelor’s degree
Language in the home English only
Day care Spanish-speaking
nanny
ADOS-1:
Communication
5
ADOS-1: Reciprocal
social interaction
9
ADOS-1 total score 14
CARS-2 rating 35
Range of concern Mild-moderate
PLS-5 2nd percentile
Note. ADOS-1 = Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule
Module 1 (Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, & Risi, 2002) for chil-
dren with few to no words; CARS-2
= Childhood Autism
Rating Scale–2; PLS-5
= Preschool Language Scale, fifth
edition. A diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder was ver-
ified using a cutoff score of 12 on the ADOS and substan-
tiated with a score of above 30 on the CARS-2. All tests
were administered by trained clinicians.
include using words to request, increasing
utterance length, and engaging in reciprocal
turn-taking using objects.
The motivation for our engagement with
this case was to change Henry’s intervention
to be consistent with the developmental
framework described earlier. Specifically,
the goal was to move away from a linguistic
approach that focuses on the Phase 2 skills
to a social approach that focuses on the five
key features, described earlier, essential to
facilitating Phase 1 sharing emotions skills.
We used an informed consent procedure
approved by the Human Subjects Institutional
Review Board (IRB) from the University of
Texas at Dallas (IRB 13-10) to obtain Henry’s
parents’ permission for him to participate in
this research.
Procedures
Alice took Henry from his usual program to
a nearby therapy room for a 30-min individual
session each day. Once a week, the session
began with my digitally recording (on an iPad
2) 10 min of Alice and Henry’s interaction.
We used these tapes in both qualitative analy-
ses of clinician–child interaction and quantita-
tive analyses of Henry’s social interaction and
communication, as described in the following
text.
Baseline phase (Weeks 1–4)
The linguistic approach, which was used
initially with Henry, served as a baseline
against which the social approach could be
compared. During the baseline sessions, Alice
followed the linguistic approach and the rec-
ommendations for Henry’s program. Specifi-
cally, she actively followed Henry’s focus of
attention, stayed on Henry’s physical level,
chose simple, high-interest, slow-paced activ-
ities, used introductory alerters (e.g., gasps,
the child’s name, “Look!” and “Wow!”) to di-
rect Henry’s attention to objects and events.
She also used pause time, simplified her lan-
guage, and commented on Henry’s actions or
focus of attention.
Social intervention phases (Weeks 5–9)
To implement the social approach, we
needed an evidence-based practice that fit our
developmental framework and, specifically,
the five key features from the clinical rec-
ommendation. Although several toddler pro-
grams are available to facilitate the first phase
of sharing emotions (see Ingersoll & Gergans,
2007; Rollins et al., 2016; Schertz & Odom,
2007; Wallace & Rogers, 2010; Wetherby
& Woods, 2006), we chose the Pathways
to Early Autism Intervention (hereafter Path-
ways; Campbell & Thibodeau Hoffman, 2014)
to guide this intervention. This selection was
made for three reasons. First, evidence had
shown Pathways to be effective in increasing
the early foundational social communication
skills of eye contact, social engagement, and
verbal reciprocity in toddlers with ASD who
are enrolled in an IDEA Part C Early Childhood
Intervention program (Rollins et al., 2016).
Second, although other toddler programs use
key features similar to those of Pathways
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
206 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2016
(e.g., face-to-face reciprocal interactions, an-
imation, imitation), to our knowledge, they
do not work directly on eye contact. The im-
portance of facilitating eye contact as an inter-
vention target has been emphasized by Jones
and Klin (2013), who suggested that early in-
tervention focusing on eye contact may build
on the neural networks that subserve early re-
flexive gaze. Finally, key features of the Path-
ways program (i.e., face-to-face social sensory
routines, eye contact, animation, and imita-
tion) outlined in Table 2 could be used to
build on the bidirectional strategies that Alice
and Henry already were using, particularly be-
cause the Pathways features are added slowly.
This helps the clinician–child dyad develop
increasingly more sophisticated levels of in-
teraction before the clinician adds more diffi-
cult key features into the interaction. For ex-
ample, as we show in the q ualitative analysis
later, Henry learned to make and maintain eye
contact through reinforcement and without
physical prompts during social sensory rou-
tines before Alice required him to respond to
animation.
Behavioral measures
To evaluate Henry’s social interaction and
communication, we coded several behavioral
measures from the 10-min recordings of
Alice and Henry’s interaction. Specifically,
the digitized recordings were coded for
(a) social engagement (i.e., simultaneously
smiling and looking at Alice’s f ace), (b)
verbal reciprocity (i.e., taking at least one
vocal/verbal turn contingent on Alice’s
vocalization/verbalization); and (c) com-
municative intention (verbal and nonverbal
combined). Communicative intention was
coded using a coding scheme described
by Ninio, Snow, Pan, and Rollins (1994),
which emphasizes socially constructed
communicative interchanges. In Ninio
et al.’s (1994) scheme, communicative intent
is coded on the level of the social interchange
(e.g., engaging with a partner in routines,
regulating another’s behavior, engaging in
discussions), acknowledging the existence of
an organization of talk at a level higher than
the single utterance (see Dore & McDermott,
1982; Streeck, 1980). A social interchange
is defined as one or more rounds of talk, all
of which serve a unitary interactive function
implicitly agreed upon by the interlocutors.
Procedure for coding videos
Social engagement, verbal reciprocity, and
communicative intentions were coded from
the digitized videos using a continuous par-
tial interval coding system (Yoder & Symons,
2010). Specifically, each 10-min video was
segmented into 5-s intervals and linked to
a computer file for later coding, using the
digitized video functions of the CHILDES
Table 2. Pathways
a
interactional strategies to facilitate Phase 1 of sharing emotions by week
and recommended key features
Week Intervention Strategies Key Feature
Week 5 1. Engage in face-to-face positioning Face-to-face
2. Create dyadic social sensory games and routines (limit
toys)
Social sensory routines
3. Reinforce eye contact without verbal or physical
prompts
Eye contact
Week 6 4. Add animation, gestures, facial expressions, and vocal
quality
Animation
Weeks 7–9 5. Add imitation of action, gestures, and sounds Imitation
a
The Pathways strategies are explained by Pathways Early Autism Intervention, by M. Campbell and R. Thibodeau
Hoffman, 2014, Unpublished manuscript, Dallas, TX. They are shared here with permission of the authors.
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Words Are Not Enough 207
utilities (MacWhinney, 1991). In continuous
partial interval coding, a behavior of interest
is coded from the video only once per interval,
regardless of how many times the behavior oc-
curs during that interval. For a 10-min video,
there were a total of 120 codable intervals,
making the total possible frequency range
for each behavior between 0 and 120. Using
this system, each 5-s interval was coded for
each measure during separate passes through
the video. All coders were blind as to week
of intervention. Two coders independently
coded 20% of the videos from Weeks 1 to 4
(linguistically based intervention) and 20%
of the videos from Weeks 5 to 9 (socially
based intervention), chosen at random. In-
terrater reliability, expressed as Cohen’s κ ,
which takes into account chance agreement
between coders, ranged from 0.65 to 1.0 for
Weeks 1–4 and 0.76 to 1.0 for Weeks 5–9,
which is considered substantial to almost per-
fect agreement (Landis & Koch, 1977).
Eye tracking
The naturalistic interactions captured on
video did not allow us to measure Henry’s
eye contact with Alice precisely, which was
a focus of the intervention. Therefore, Alice
brought Henry into the eye-tracking labora-
tory on two separate occasions. The first time
(Time 1) was during the baseline phase, and
the second (Time 2) was a few days after
the Week 9 session. The eye-tracking labo-
ratory was equipped with a SensoMotoric In-
struments (SMI) RED-m portable eye-tracking
laboratory, with a sampling rate of 60 and
120 Hz. The eye tracker was used to record
and quantify Henry’s eye movements as an
indirect measure of eye contact. To collect
the eye-tracking data, Henry sat on Alice’s lap
in front of a 15-in. LCD display. The video
stimulus was a female actor who was looking
directly into the camera and was engaged in
childhood songs (e.g., “Peek-a-boo,” “Wheels
on the Bus”) to simulate a dyadic interaction.
The scene was filmed in front of distractor
toys. The video was presented using a lap-
top that ran SMI’s Experiment Center soft-
ware. The eye tracker was first focused and
calibrated. During the calibration, Henry was
shown images of an animated and concen-
tric circle that appeared in one of five lo-
cations on the screen. Quality of calibration
was verified by the deviations on the x-and
y-axes. If any deviation was larger than 1
,
the calibration procedure was repeated. The
SMI BeGaze video analysis package was used
to draw dynamic areas of interest (AOIs) on
each frame of the video to ensure continuous
motion within the AOIs. The BeGaze program
calculated the total fixation time and the per-
centage of visual fixation time to each set of
eyes, mouth + nose, body, all toys, and out-
side the region of AOIs.
In addition, we compared Henry’s eye-
tracking results with the results for Jena, a
typically developing child who matched on
age (29 months), mother’s age (34 years),
and language in the home (English). Jena
was enrolled in English-speaking day care and
scored in the 96th percentile on the Preschool
Language Scale (fifth edition). Jena’s mother
brought her to the eye-tracking laboratory one
time. All eye-tracking procedures used to col-
lect, record, and quantify Jens’s eye move-
ments were the same as those for Henry, ex-
cept that Jena sat on her mother’s lap.
Results
This section presents the results of three
analyses. The first is a qualitative analysis that
uses excerpts from the 10-min digitized videos
to elucidate Alice and Henry’s interaction
during the baseline and social intervention
phases. The qualitative analysis is followed by
two quantitative analyses of the results based
on the behavioral measures and eye-tracking
data.
Qualitative results
The qualitative results are presented in
terms of five excerpts. The excerpts begin
at baseline and span 8 weeks. Excerpts 2-5
illustrate key features from Table 2.
Excerpt 1: Baseline (linguistic intervention)
In this excerpt from Week 4, which il-
lustrates the linguistic intervention, Henry
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
208 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2016
and Alice are positioned next to each other,
building a block tower at a table. Alice has a
pile of blocks in front of her. Henry is stand-
ing, and Alice is kneeling.
Alice turns her head to the side to look at Henry
and says, “Henry!” while holding up a block.
Henry looks at the block in Alice’s hand and then
looks down at the floor.
Alice, still looking at Henry, says “Oh!”
Henry looks at the block in Alice’s hand, grabs it,
and says, “More block.”
Henry looks at the block tower and places the block
on top.
Alice looks at the pile blocks and says, “You want
another one?”
Alice looks at Henry and holds up a white block
and a pink block.
Henry continues to look at the block tower and
vocalizes to himself as he adjusts the pieces.
Henry looks at the pile of blocks in front of Alice.
Alice looks at Henry, holds up two blocks, and says,
“What color?”
Henry looks at the white block and tries to take it.
Alice, still looking at Henry, holds the blocks higher
and again says, “What color?”
Henry continues to look at the white block in
Alice’s hand, points to it and says, “White.”
Alice hands the block to Henry and says, “You want
the white block?”
Henry looks at the tower in front of him as he adds
the white block and says, “White.”
Alice looks at the tower and says, “I’ll put one on
top,” as she adds the block to the tower.
Henry, still looking at the tower, adjusts the block
that Alice added and vocalizes to himself.
Alice picks up a block from the table.
Henry, looking at the tower, jumps and smiles
while he vocalizes.
Alice slides the block to Henry and says, “Now
you.”
Henry looks at the tower as he adds the block to
the top.
In this excerpt, Alice chose simple, high-
interest, slow-paced activities, knelt to be on
Henry’s physical level, used alerters to di-
rect his attention to the block, simplified her
language, and used pause time to facilitate
Henry’s word use and turn-taking with an ob-
ject, which were the targets of the linguistic
approach. In response, Henry labeled and re-
quested as well as allowed Alice to take her
turn in building the tower. Although he ap-
peared happy, he never looked at Alice’s face,
as would be expected for a child with so-
cial understanding. Instead, he gazed at the
blocks and block tower for the entirety of the
interaction.
Excerpt 2: Social intervention, using
face-to-face, social sensory routines and eye
contact
In this excerpt from Week 5, Alice’s goals
are to position herself so that she is sitting
face to face with Henry while engaging him in
social sensory routines and reinforcing his eye
contactwithoutverbalorphysicalprompts.
Henry is sitting in an adult desk chair, and
Alice is sitting on the floor below him. He is
requesting that Alice spin him in the chair.
Alice spins the chair around one time and then
stops it in front of her.
Alice looks up at Henry and tries to look into his
eyes.
Henry looks at the arm of the chair as he touches
it.
Alice looks up at Henry’s face and tries again to
look into his eyes.
Alice moves Henry’s hand from the arm of the chair
and says, “Oh hand, comes off.” Henry continues
to look at and touch the arm of the chair and says,
“Go.”
Alice looks up at Henry until she is able to look
into his eyes. As soon as she looks into his eyes, she
spins the chair and says, “Spinning in the chair.”
Alice stops the chair in front of her, looks up at
Henry, and tries to look into his eyes.
Henry goes back to looking at the arm of the chair
and says, “Go.”
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Words Are Not Enough 209
Alice continues trying to look into Henry’s eyes,
but Henry averts his gaze.
Henry says, “Go, go.”
Alice continues to look up at Henry and is now
able to look into his eyes; she immediately spins
the chair and says, “Spinning the chair.”
Alice stops the chair in front of her.
Henry looks at Alice’s eyes, smiles, and says, “Go.”
Alice spins the chair and says, “Again.”
Alice stops the chair in front of herself.
Henry looks at Alice’s eyes, smiles, and says, “Go.”
This excerpt illustrates the three key fea-
tures used in Week 5 (i.e., face-to-face social
sensory routines and eye contact) that Alice
had learned. Rather than sitting beside Henry,
as was seen in Excerpt 1, Alice sat face to face
with him, placing herself in his line of sight
whenever possible. Because Henry tended to
look down, averting his gaze, Alice sat below
him and looked up. She concentrated on find-
ing a motivating sensory routine without toys
(i.e., spinning in the chair). To reinforce eye
contactwithoutverbalorphysicalprompts,
Alice moved her face in line with Henry’s to
look into his eyes. She quickly reinforced the
brief eye contact by spinning him in the chair.
Henry soon understood that he needed to look
at Alice’s eyes to be spun and began to inte-
grate looking at Alice’s eyes with a smile (i.e.,
social engagement) and the word “go.”
Excerpt 3: Social intervention, using
face-to-face, social sensory routines, eye
contact, and animation
In this Week 6 excerpt, Alice adds anima-
tion to her repertoire of strategies. She con-
tinues to use face-to-face positioning when
engaging Henry in routines and quickly re-
inforces eye contact. By Week 6, however,
Henry is more socially engaged, as this ex-
cerpt illustrates. Alice and Henry are facing
each other about 4 ft apart; Henry sits in a
cube chair, and Alice kneels to be on his phys-
ical level. They are rolling a large therapy ball
back and forth. Henry has the ball.
Alice looks at Henry’s eyes and says, “Roll the ball
to....
Henry looks at Alice’s eyes, gives a big smile, and
says, “Alice.”
Alice, still looking at Henry’s eyes, opens her arms
wide and laughs with an exaggerated smile.
Henry, still looking at Alice eyes, rolls the ball with
a big smile.
Alice, still looking at Henry’s eyes, smiles and
laughs while she catches the ball.
Henry and Alice look at each other, smiling and
laughing.
In Excerpt 3, Alice was more animated than
in earlier sessions when she was concentrat-
ing on reinforcing eye contact. Here, Henry
and Alice continuously looked at each other’s
eyes, which registered positive affect. They
laughed and smiled as they rolled the ball to
each other. Henry used words within the rou-
tine (e.g., “Alice”) and was socially engaged
(e.g., looked at Alice’s eyes and smiled).
Excerpt 4: Social intervention, using
face-to-face, social sensory routines, eye
contact, animation, and imitation
In Week 7, Alice adds imitation to her reper-
toire of strategies. She looks for opportunities
to imitate actions, gestures, and sounds. Be-
cause the interactional strategies are cumula-
tive, she is able to use all of the strategies to-
gether, as illustrated in this excerpt. Alice and
Henry are about 4 ft apart, facing each other;
Henry sits in a cube chair, and Alice kneels
to be on his physical level. They are rolling
a large therapy ball back and forth. Alice has
the ball.
Henry looks down briefly and laughs and smiles as
he puts his hand to his cheek.
Henry looks at Alice’s eyes and vocalizes.
Alice looks at Henry and says, “Oh!” while imitating
his hand positioning.
Alice, still looking at Henry’s eyes, says, “You’re
silly.”
Alice, still looking at Henry’s eyes, says, “Roll the
ball,” and pats the ball.
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
210 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2016
Alice, still looking at Henry’s eyes, says, “Roll the
ball.”
Henry, still looking at Alice’s eyes, says, “To Alice.”
Alice looks at and points to Henry, and says, “To
....
Henry continues looking at Alice’s eyes and says,
“Henry.”
Alice still looking at Henry’s eyes, says, “To Henry.”
Alice, still looking at Henry’s eyes, rolls the ball to
Henry, as she gasps and says, “Ahhh!”
Henry looks at Alice, smiles, and repeats, “Ahhh!”
while catching the ball.
Alice looks at Henry’s eyes and laughs.
Henry looks at Alice’s eyes and makes vocaliza-
tions.
Alice continues looking at Henry and imitates his
vocalizations.
To summarize, in Excerpt 4, Alice imitated
Henry’s nonverbal actions (hand positioning)
and vocalizations. Henry continued to use
words within the routine and was socially en-
gaged (e.g., looked at Alice’s eyes and smiled).
He also started to imitate Alice’s vocalizations.
Excerpt 5: Social intervention, using
face-to-face, social sensory routines, eye
contact, animation, and imitation
In subsequent sessions, Alice continued to
use all of the interactional strategies from the
previous weeks. Henry appeared to be more
socially engaged, and their vocal play was
more reciprocal, as Excerpt 5 from Week 8
illustrates. Alice and Henry sat face to face. Al-
ice sat on the floor and Henry in a cube chair.
Henry looks away from Alice and vocalizes.
Alice looks at Henry’s eyes and imitates the vocal-
ization.
Henry gasps, looks at Alice’s eyes, and makes a
raspberry.
Alice looks into Henry’s eyes and gasps exaggerat-
edly.
Henry looks at Alice’s eyes, gasps, and makes a
raspberry.
Alice looks into Henry’s eyes, gasps, makes a
raspberry, and then laughs.
Henry looks away from Alice’s eyes, smiling.
Henry looks at Alice’s eyes and makes a raspberry.
As Excerpt 5 illustrates, Henry continued to
be socially engaged with Alice. He appeared
to show progress in social communication
each week.
Quantitative results
The quantitative results are presented in
terms of the behavioral measures and eye-
tracking data.
Behavioral measures
Henry’s progress in terms of changes in
behavioral measures is displayed in Figures
2 and 3. The number of 5-s intervals dur-
ing which a behavior was exhibited (y-axis)
was graphed by the week number (x-axis).
Thebaselinedataforthesecomparisonswere
gathered during the use of the linguistic ap-
proach (Weeks 1–4) before beginning the so-
cial approach. When Alice began using a so-
cial approach to intervention (Weeks 5–9),
Henry appeared to make progress on his use
of social engagement, verbal reciprocity, and
intentions within routines. Visual inspection
of the graphs in Figure 2 shows a clear pos-
itive slope for social engagement and verbal
reciprocity. It is noteworthy that intentions
within routines appeared to decline from the
first week to the last week of social inter-
vention (negative slope). Nonetheless, Henry
used more intentions within routines when
Alice used a social-cognitive approach to in-
tervention (Weeks 5–9) as compared with a
linguistic approach (Weeks 1–4). Henry made
progress in his use of intentions to regulate an-
other’s behavior when Alice used a linguistic
approach, which continued through Week 5,
when he was using eye contact to request ac-
tion in a sensory game. Regulating Alice’s be-
havior, however, declined as the intervention
became more social (Figure 3).
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Words Are Not Enough 211
Figure 2. Henry’s performance on social engagement, verbal reciprocity, and engagement in social
routines across linguistic (Weeks 1–4) and social (Weeks 5–9) approaches to intervention.
Figure 3. Henry’s performance on intentions to regulate another’s behavior across linguistic (Weeks 1–4)
and social (Weeks 5–9) approaches to interventions.
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
212 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2016
Eye tracking
We found that Henry’s percentage of fix-
ation time to eyes, mouth, body, and toys
changed from Time 1 to Time 2 (Table 3). The
percentage of time that Henry looked at the
eyes increased from 11.4% at Time 1 to 41.4%
at Time 2. The latter percentage is remark-
ably similar to the amount of time that Jena
looked at the eyes of the actor in the video.
The amount of time that Henry looked at the
mouth + nose area and the body decreased
with the intervention. In addition, he spent
more time looking outside the region of AIOs.
Although this is only an indirect measure, it
suggests that Henry’s eye contact improved.
SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION
In this article, I elucidated the unfolding
of the three phases of cognitive development
through which typical children move during
the first 2 years of life and described the inter-
relationships between cognition, communica-
tive intention, word learning, and language (as
illustrated in Figure 1). In addition, I explained
that the social-cognitive trajectory necessary
for social communication and language con-
sists of sharing emotions and sharing atten-
tion and intention but that sharing percep-
tions and pursuing goals are not parts of the
social-cognitive trajectory (Tomasello et al.,
2005).
Historically, the second phase of sharing
perceptions and pursuing goals has not been
well understood or elaborated on, perhaps
because sharing perceptions and pursuing
goals are interwoven with sharing emotions
and sharing attention and intentions in
typically developing children (Tomasello
et al., 2005). Children with ASD, however,
often develop Phase 2 cognition, sharing
perceptions and pursuing goals, without the
neighboring social abilities. Cognitive skills
related to sharing perceptions and pursuing
goals allow children with ASD to do the
following: (a) be goal directed and persistent;
(b) understand that others have goals and
perceptually monitor their behavior; (c) pre-
dict what comes next; and (d) take turns in
an interaction that involves objects. Despite
the sophistication in understanding many
of these nonsocial-cognitive skills, these
children appear to lack the social cognition
necessary to understand mutual knowledge
and that they are in a relationship in which
their attention and intentions can be shared.
Interventions, therefore, need to be designed
to support these social developmental
advances.
Children with ASD who are in the phase of
sharing perceptions and pursuing goals can-
not yet use social cues or understand their
partner’s intentions to learn language. Rather,
these children use early perceptional word-
learning strategies, which limit them to iso-
lated words and phrases to label or regu-
late the behaviors of others. Unfortunately,
these peripheral language forms do not read-
ily progress to true social communication
and language (Rollins & Snow, 1998). Social
communication and creative language require
Table 3. Percentage of eye fixation on AOIs and outside the AOI for Henry at two time points
a
and for Jena, a typically developing child
Eye Mouth + Nose Body All Toys Outside AOI
Henry (Time 1) 11.7 46.0 11.7 8.4 22.2
Henry (Time 2) 41.4 3.8 0 0 54.8
Jena 41.0 18.3 14.2 0 26.5
Note.AOI= area of interest.
a
Henry was brought to the eye-tracking laboratory during baseline (Time 1) and, again, a few days after the Week 9
session (Time 2).
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Words Are Not Enough 213
the foundational social-cognitive skill of
shared emotions and shared attention and in-
tention (Tomasello et al., 2005).
On the basis of this theoretical framework,
I made the clinical recommendation that in-
terventionists who work with children in the
second phase of sharing perceptions and pur-
suing goals focus their interventions efforts
on facilitating the sharing emotions skills of
Phase 1, as these are the critical precursors
of social communication. In particular, I rec-
ommended five key features essential to fa-
cilitate Phase 1 sharing emotions skills: (a)
position yourself face to face with the child;
(b) engage in social sensory routines, limiting
the use of toys whenever possible; (c) estab-
lish eye contact without verbal and physical
prompts, making sure to reinforce eye con-
tact immediately; (d) use animation; and (e)
use contingent imitation of the child gestures,
vocalization, and words. To support these rec-
ommendations, I presented a clinical case in
which we used Pathways, a social approach
to intervention, to slowly add and integrate all
five of the Phase 1 key features.
The child in the case example was Henry,
a 29-month-old toddler with ASD who was in
the phase of sharing perceptions and pursuing
goals. He used verbal and nonverbal means to
request and label items in his environment.
He rarely looked at his communicative part-
ner, focusing his gaze on the objects around
him. Because Henry was enrolled in a linguis-
tically based program, his initial goals focused
on peripheral language forms and engaging
in turn-taking with objects. The problem was
that Henry’s program created an environment
that perpetuated the nonsocial language skills
that he already was capable of producing.
After 4 weeks of linguistically based inter-
vention, Henry’s graduate-student clinician,
Alice, and I changed Henry’s intervention to
a social approach, using Pathways. This pro-
vided a naturalistic baseline of data against
which we could measure progress that cor-
responded with introduction of Pathways so-
cial approach. Alice learned to engage Henry
in eye contact and social engagement using
the five key features of face-to-face position-
ing, dyadic social sensory routines, reinforc-
ing eye contact without verbal or physical
prompts, and adding animation and imitation
(as summarized in Table 2). Once the social in-
tervention was initiated, Henry demonstrated
an increase in his sharing emotions capabili-
ties, as demonstrated by increased social en-
gagement and verbal reciprocity (illustrated
in Figure 2). I n addition, his communicative
intentions became more social when inten-
tion within social routines was added to his
repertoire (illustrated in Figure 3). This addi-
tion is commensurate with the developmental
trajectory of social communication in typical
children. That is, although requests emerge
early in typically developing children (as pro-
toimperatives; Bates et al., 1975), social par-
ticipation acts are more frequent (Snow, Pan,
Imbens-Bailey, & Herman, 1996). Social rou-
tines precede joint attention (Ninio & Snow,
1996; Rollins, in press) and provide the con-
texts for a shared mutual knowledge (Rollins,
2014).
Because we were unable to measure eye
contact objectively, we used eye-tracking
technology to obtain an indirect measure.
For this purpose, Henry was brought into an
eye-tracking laboratory once during baseline
(Time 1) and, again, a few days after the Week
9 intervention session (Time 2). The results of
the eye-tracking data suggest that, although
Henry spent more time looking outside the
AOIs (eyes, mouth + nose, body, and toys) at
Time 2, the percentage of time that he looked
at the eyes of the actor in the video increased
from 11.4% at Time 1 to 41.4% at Time 2.
The latter was commensurate with a typical
peer matched for chronological age and home
language.
The importance of facilitating eye contact
in interventions with children on the spec-
trum has been emphasized by Jones and Klin
(2013). They found that, despite the pres-
ence of early reflexive gaze to adult eyes,
infants with ASD exhibit a decline in eye
gaze between 2 and 6 months. Jones and
Klin suggested that infants with ASD who do
not engage in eye contact miss opportunities
to engage in social reciprocity and begin to
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
214 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2016
favor the object world. Jones and Klin sug-
gested that early intervention that focuses
on eye contact could build on the neural
networks that subserve early reflexive gaze,
which could have cascading effects on the de-
velopment of social communication, as well
as result in positive collateral changes in other
areas of development. This case example was
consistent this hypothesis.
It is noteworthy that this case example was
not a study with experimental control. There
are many threats to the internal validity of
this case example, including that Henry may
have shown improvement simply because he
had more time to develop, so that changes
could be due to simple maturation. The find-
ings of changes contingent on introducing the
socially based intervention, however, were
consistent with those of Rollins et al. (2016),
which differed in that the intervention was de-
livered by parents within an authentic home
setting. These two reports are consistent in
supporting the use of a social-cognitive ap-
proach over a linguistic approach for children
with ASD who seem to be stuck in the phase
of sharing perceptions and pursuing goals.
Early identification and intervention are
known to have positive implications for a
child’s level of functioning and school place-
ment (Dawson et al., 2010; Reichow, 2012;
Warren et al., 2011). It is imperative, however,
that interventionists providing early interven-
tion understand a child’s social-cognitive func-
tioning and not be deceived by peripheral
language forms. This case example provided
added support for the notion that evidence-
based practices used with young children
with ASD are most effective when grounded
in a comprehensive framework that is sensi-
tive to the child’s social-cognitive abilities.
REFERENCES
Adamson, L., & Russell, C. (1999). Emotion regulation
and the emergence of joint attention. In P. Rochat
(Ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others
in the first months of life (pp. 281–295). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. (1984). Coordinating at-
tention to people and objects in mother–infant
and peer–infant interaction. Child Development, 55,
1278–1289.
Baldwin, D. (1993). Early referential understanding: In-
fants’ ability to recognize referential acts for what they
are. Developmental Psychology, 29, 832–843.
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., &
Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cog-
nition and communication in infancy. New York:
Academic Press.
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisi-
tion of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly, 21, 205–224.
Bates, E., & Goodman, J. (1997). On the inseparability
of grammar and the lexicon: Evidence from acquisi-
tion, aphasia and real-time processing. Language and
Cognitive Processes, 12(5–6), 507–586.
Bates, E., & Goodman, J. (1999). On the emergence of
grammar from the lexicon. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.),
The emergence of language (pp. 29–79). Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Bateson, M. C. (1975). Mother–infant exchanges: The
epigenesis of conversation interaction. Annals of the
New York Academy of Sciences, 263, 101–113.
Brazelton, T. B., Koslowski, B., & Main, M. (1974). The
origins of reciprocity: The early mother-infant interac-
tion. In M. Lewis, & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The effect
of the infant on its caregiver (pp. 49–76). New York:
Wiley.
Camaioni, L. (1993). The development of intentional
communication: A re-analysis. In J. Nadel, & L.
Camaioni (Eds.), New perspectives in early com-
munication development (pp. 82–96). New York:
Routledge.
Campbell, M., & Thibodeau Hoffman, R. (2014).
Pathways early autism intervention. Unpublished
manuscript, Dallas, TX: Pathways Early Intervention.
Carpenter, M., Nagel, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social
cognition, joint attention, and communicative com-
petence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of
the Society for Research in Child Development, 63
(4, Serial No. 225).
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2014). Facts
about ASDs. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/
ncbddd/autism/data.html
Charman, T. (2003). Why is joint attention a pivotal
skill in autism? Philosophical Transactions of the
Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sci-
ences, 358(1430), 315–324.
Dawson, G., Rogers, S., Munson, J., Smith, M., Win-
ter, J., Greenson, J., et al. (2010). Randomized, con-
trolled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism:
The Early Start Denver Model. Pediatrics, 125(1),
e17–e23.
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Words Are Not Enough 215
Dore, J., & McDermott, R. P. (1982). Linguistic indeter-
minacy and social context in utterance interpretation.
Language, 58, 374–398.
Gillespie-Lynch, K., Khalulyan, A., del Rosario, M.,
McCarthy, B., Gomez, L., Sigman, M., et al. (2013). Is
early joint attention associated with school-age prag-
matic language? Autism, 19(2), 168–177.
Greenspan, S. I., & Shanker, S. (2004). The first idea:
How symbols, language and intelligence evolved
from our primate ancestors to modern human.Cam-
bridge, MA: Da Capo Press.
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Hennon, E. A., &
Maguire, M. J. (2004). Hybrid theories at the fron-
tier of developmental psychology: The emergentist
coalition model of word learning as a case in point.
In G. Hall, & S. Waxman (Eds.), Weaving a lexicon
(pp. 173–204). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hollich, G. (2000).
An emergentist coalition model for word learning:
Mapping words to objects is a product of the in-
teraction of multiple cues. In R. M. Golinkoff, K.
Hirsh-Pasek,L.Bloom,L.Smith,A.Woodward,N.
Akhtar, M. Tomasello,
... G. Hollich (Eds.), Becom-
ing a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisi-
tion (pp. 136–164). New York: Oxford University
Press.
Hollich, G., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Brand, R.
J., Brown, E., Chung, H. L., et al. (2000). Breaking
the language barrier: An emergentist coalition model
for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the
Society for Research in Child Development, 65(3),
1–135.
Ingersoll, B., & Gergans, S. (2007). The effect of a
parent-implemented imitation intervention on sponta-
neous imitation skills in young children with autism.
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 28(2),
163–175.
Johnson, M. H., & Morton, J. (1991). Biology and cog-
nitive development: The case of face recognition.
Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present
but in decline in 2–6-month-old infants later diag-
nosed with autism. Nature, 504(7480), 427–431.
doi:10.1038/nature12715
Kasari, C., Paparella, T., Freeman, S., & Jahromi, L.
(2008). Language outcome in autism: Randomized
comparison of joint attention and play interventions.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76,
125–137.
Landis, J., & Koch, G. (1977). The measurement of ob-
server agreement for categorical data. Biometrics,
33(1), 159–174.
Legerstee, M. (1991). The role of person and object in elic-
iting early imitation. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 51, 423–433.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (2002).
Autism diagnostic observation schedule (ADOS).Los
Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
MacWhinney, B. (1991). The CHILDES project: Computa-
tional tools for analyzing talk. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Maurer, D. (1985). Infants’ perception of faces. In T. Field,
& M. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 73–
100). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn in-
fants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development,
54(3), 702–709.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1989). Imitation in new-
born infants: Exploring the range o f gestures imitated
and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psy-
chology, 25(6), 954–962.
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudi-
nal study of joint attention and language development
in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Develop-
mental Disorders, 20(1), 115–128.
Mundy, P., & Thorp, D. (2008). The neural basis of
early joint-attention behavior. In T. Charman, & W.
Stone (Eds.), Social & communication development
in autism spectrum disorders: Early identification,
diagnosis & intervention (pp. 296–336). New York:
The Guilford Press.
Ninio, A., & Snow, C. E. (1996). Pragmatic development.
Boulder, CO: Westview.
Ninio, A., Snow, C. E., Pan, B. A., & Rollins, P. R. (1994).
Classifying communicative acts in children’s interac-
tions. Journal of Communication Disorders, 27(2),
157–187.
Reichow, B. (2012). Overview of meta-analyses on early
intensive behavioral intervention for young children
with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 42(4), 512–520.
Rochat, P., & Striano, T. (1999). Social cognitive develop-
ment in the first year. In P. Rochat (Ed.), Early social
cognition (pp. 3–34). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rollins, P. R. (1999). Early pragmatic accomplishments
and vocabulary development in preschool children
with autism. American Journal of Speech-Language
Pathology, 8(2), 181–190.
Rollins, P. R. (2003). Caregiver contingent comments and
subsequent vocabulary comprehension. Applied Psy-
cholinguistics, 24, 221–234.
Rollins, P. R. (2014). Facilitating early social commu-
nication skills: From theory to practice. Shawnee
Mission, KS: AAPC Publishing.
Rollins, P. R. (in press). Developmental pragmatics. In
Y. Huang (Ed.), Handbook of pragmatics. Oxford,
England: Oxford University Press.
Rollins, P. R., Campbell, M., Hoffman, R. T., & Self, K.
(2016). A community-based early intervention pro-
gram for toddlers with autism spectrum disorders.
Autism: International Journal of Research and Prac-
tice, 20(2), 219–232.
Rollins, P. R., & Greenwald, L. C. (2013). Affect attune-
ment during mother–infant interaction: How specific
intensities predict the stability of infants’ joint atten-
tion. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 32(4),
339–366.
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
216 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2016
Rollins, P. R., & Snow, C. E. (1998). Shared attention and
grammatical development in typical children and chil-
dren with autism. Journal of Child Language, 25(3),
653–673.
Rollins, P. R., & Trautman, C. H. (2011). Caregiver input
before joint attention: The role of multimodal input.
Paper presented at the International Congress for the
Study of Child Language (IASCL), Baltimore, MD.
Rollins, P. R., Wambacq, I., Dowell, D., Mathews, L., &
Reese, P. B. (1998). An intervention technique for
children with autistic spectrum disorder: Joint atten-
tional routines. Journal of Communication Disor-
ders, 31(2), 181–193.
Schertz, H., & Odom, S. (2007). Promoting joint attention
in toddlers with autism: A parent-mediated develop-
mental model. Journal of Autism & Developmental
Disorders, 37(8), 1562–1575.
Snow, C. E. (1977). The development of conversation
between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Lan-
guage, 4, 1–22.
Snow, C. E., Pan, B. A., Imbens-Bailey, A., & Herman,
J. (1996). Learning how to say what one means: A
longitudinal study of children’s speech act use. Social
Development, 5, 56–84.
Spitz, R. A. (1965). The first year of life: A psychoanalytic
study of normal and deviant development of object
relations. New York: International Universities Press.
Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the in-
fant: View from psychoanalysis and development
psychology. New York: Basic Books.
Streeck, J. (1980). Speech acts in interaction: A critique
of Searle. Discourse Processes, 3, 133–154.
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition.
In C. Moore, & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention:
Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–130).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human
cognition. Boston, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tomasello, M., & Barton, M. (1994). Learning words in
nonostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology,
30, 639–650.
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intention-
ality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121–125.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll,
H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The
origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 28(05), 675–691.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A
new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78,
705–722.
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Instincts for human understanding
and for cultural cooperation: Their development in
infancy. In M. V. Cranach, K. Foppa, W. Lepenies,
& D. Ploog (Eds.), Human ethnology: Claims and
limits of a new discipline (pp. 530–571). Cambridge,
England: Cambridge University Press.
Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. (2001). Infant intersubjectiv-
ity: Research, theory and clinical applications. Journal
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
, 42, 3–48.
Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersub-
jectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning
in the first year. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gestures and
symbol (pp. 183–229). London: Academic Press.
Vouloumanos, A., Hauser, M. D., Werker, J. F., & Martin,
A. (2010). The tuning of human neonates’ preference
for speech. Child Development, 81(2), 517–527.
Wallace, K. S., & Rogers, S. J. (2010). Intervening in
infancy: Implications for autism spectrum disorders.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(12),
1300–1320.
Warren, Z., McPheeters, M. L., Sathe, N., Foss-Feig, J.
H., Glasser, A., & Veenstra-VanderWeele, J. (2011).
A systematic review of early intensive intervention
for autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 127(5),
e1303–e1311.
Wetherby, A. M., & Woods, J. J. (2006). Early social in-
teraction project for children with autism spectrum
disorders beginning in the second year of life a pre-
liminary study. Topics in Early Childhood Special Ed-
ucation, 26(2), 67–82.
Wetherby, A. M., Yonclas, D. G., & Bryan, A. A.
(1989). Communicative profiles of preschool chil-
dren with handicaps: Implications for early identifica-
tion. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54,
148–158.
Wolff, P. H. (1987). The development of behavioral
states and the expression of emotions in early in-
fancy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Yoder, P., & Symons, F. J. (2010). Observational mea-
surement of behavior. New York: Springer.
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.